Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 5
  • Print publication year: 2007
  • Online publication date: June 2012

Chapter 9 - Behavioral, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological approaches to implicit perception

from Part I - The cognitive science of consciousness

References

Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000). Parts outweigh the whole (word) in unconscious analysis of meaning. Psychological Science, 11(2), 118–124.
Abrams, R. L., Klinger, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Subliminal words activate semantic categories (not automated motor responses). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(1), 100–106.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Fear and the human amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 5879–5891.
Azzopardi, P., & Cowey, A. (1998). Blindsight and visual awareness. Consciousness & Cognition, 7(3), 292–311.
Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bar, M., & Biederman, I. (1998). Subliminal visual priming. Psychological Science, 9(6), 464–469.
Bernat, E., Bunce, S., & Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 11–34.
Bernat, E., Shevrin, H., & Snodgrass, M. (2001). Subliminal visual oddball stimuli evoke a P300 component. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(1), 159–171.
Berti, A., Allport, A., Driver, J., Dienes, Z., Oxbury, J., & Oxbury, S. (1992). Levels of processing for visual stimuli in an “extinguished” field. Neuropsychologia, 30, 403–415.
Brazdil, M., Rektor, I., Dufek, M., Jurak, P., & Daniel, P. (1998). Effect of subthreshold target stimuli on event-related potentials. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 107, 64–68.
Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., et al. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17, 875–887.
Brown, C., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 34–44.
Brown, M., & Besner, D. (2002). Semantic priming: On the role of awareness in visual word recognition in the absence of an expectancy. Consciousness and Cognition, 11(3), 402–422.
Campion, J., Latto, R., & Smith, Y. M. (1983). Is blindsight an effect of scattered light, spared cortex, and near-threshold vision?Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 6(3), 423–486.
Carr, T. H., & Dagenbach, D. (1990). Semantic priming and repetition priming from masked words: Evidence for a center-surround attentional mechanism in perceptual recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 341–350.
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. M. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 36(4), 387–395.
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments upon the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.
Cohen, A., Ivry, R. B., Rafal, R. D., & Kohn, C. (1995). Activating response codes by stimuli in the neglected visual field. Neuropsychology, 9, 165–173.
Coles, M. G., Gratton, G., & Donchin, E. (1988). Detecting early communication: Using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing. Biological Psychology, 26, 69–89.
Corteen, R. S., & Dunn, D. (1974). Shock-associated words in a nonattended message: A test for momentary awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(6), 1143–1144.
Corteen, R. S., & Wood, B. (1972). Autonomic responses to shock-associated words in an unattended channel. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(3), 308–313.
Cowey, A., & Stoerig, P. (1995). Blindsight in monkeys. Nature, 373, 247–249.
Damian, M. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 154–165.
Daza, M. T., Ortells, J. J., & Fox, E. (2002). Perception without awareness: Further evidence from a Stroop priming task. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(8), 1316–1324.
Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Yang, C., & Nagata, M. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: Evidence that N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 137–146.
Deacon, D., & Shelley-Tremblay, J. (2000). How automatically is meaning accessed: A review of the effects of attention on semantic processing. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, E82–94.
Debner, J., & Jacoby, L. (1994). Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness and control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(2), 304–317.
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le, B., D., Mangin, J., Poline, J., et al. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4(7), 752–758.
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clech, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597–600.
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 487–506.
Dell’Acqua, R., & Grainger, J. (1999). Unconscious semantic priming from pictures. Cognition, 73(1), B1–B15.
Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70(1), 80–90.
Devrim, M., Demiralp, T., & Kurt, A. (1997). The effects of subthreshold visual stimulation on P300 response. Neuroreport, 8, 3113–3117.
Dosher, B. A. (1998). The response-window regression method – some problematic assumptions – comment on Draine and Greenwald (1998). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(3), 311–317.
Draine, S. C., & Greenwald, A. G. (1998). Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(3), 286–303.
Driver, J., & Mattingley, J. B. (1998). Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 17–22.
Driver, J., & Vuilleumier, P. (2001). Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction. Cognition, 79(1–2), 39–88.
Driver, J., Vuilleumier, P., Eimer, M., & Rees, G. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and evoked potential correlates of conscious and unconscious vision in parietal extinction patients. Neuroimage, 14, S68–75.
Dulany, D. E. (2004). Higher order representation in a mentalistic metatheory. In R. J. Gennaro (Ed.), Higher-order theories of consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101(2–3), 293–313.
Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.
Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 514–520.
Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392, 598–601.
Esterman, M., McGlinchey-Berroth, R., Verfaellie, M., Grande, L., Kilduff, P., & Milberg, W. (2002). Aware and unaware perception in hemispatial neglect: Evidence from a stem completion priming task. Cortex, 38(2), 233–246.
Farah, M. J., Monheit, M. A., & Wallace, M. A. (1991). Unconscious perception of “extinguished” visual stimuli: Reassessing the evidence. Neuropsychologia, 29, 949–958.
Fendrich, R., Wessinger, C. M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1992). Residual vision in a scotoma: Implications for blindsight. Science, 258, 1489–1491.
Fendrich, R., Wessinger, C. M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1993). Sources of blindsight – Reply to Stoerig and Weiskrantz. Science, 261, 493–495.
Fodor, J. (1986). Modularity of mind. In Z. W. Pylyshyn & W. Demopoulos (Ed.), Meaning and cognitive structure: Issues in the computational theory of mind (pp. 129–167). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Freud, S. (1966). Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton.
Gazzaniga, M. S., Fendrich, R., & Wessinger, C. M. (1994). Blindsight reconsidered. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(3), 93–96.
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25.
Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349, 154–156.
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New Look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed. American Psychologist, 47, 766–779.
Greenwald, A., Abrams, R., Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). Long-term semantic memory versus contextual memory in unconscious number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 235–247.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(4–27).
Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699–1702.
Greenwald, A. G., Schuh, E. S., & Klinger, M. R. (1995). Activation by marginally perceptible (subliminal) stimuli: Dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(1), 22–42.
Greenwald, A. G., Spangenberg, E. R., Pratkanis, A. R., & Eskenazi, J. (1991). Double-blind tests of subliminal self-help audiotapes. Psychological Science, 2(119–122).
Hannula, D. E., Simons, D. J., & Cohen, N. J. (2005). Imaging implicit perception: Promise and pitfalls. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 247–255.
Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–66.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic and intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory & Language, 30, 513–541.
Jakobson, L. S., & Goodale, M. A. (1991). Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: A kinematic analysis of human prehension. Experimental Brain Research, 8, 199–208.
Jaskowski, P., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Schlotterbeck, E., & Verleger, R. (2002). Traces left on visual selective attention by stimuli that are not consciously identified. Psychological Science, 13(1), 48–54.
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311.
Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(1), 27–39.
Kiefer, M., & Spitzer, M. (2000). Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activation. Neuroreport, 11(11), 2401–2407.
Klapp, S., & Hinkley, L. (2002). The negative compatibility effect: Unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 255–269.
Klinger, M., Burton, P., & Pitts, G. (2000). Mechanisms of unconscious priming: Ⅰ. Response competition, not spreading activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(2), 441–455.
Koechlin, E., Naccache, L., Block, E., & Dehaene, S. (1999). Primed numbers: Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25(6), 1882–1905.
Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205.
Ladavas, E., Paladini, R., & Cubelli, R. (1993). Implicit associative priming in a patient with left visual neglect. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1307–1320.
Lane, R. D., & Nadel, L. (2000). Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.
Le Doux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Shuster.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1965). On binocular rivalry. Assen, The Netherlands: Royal VanGorcum.
Levin, D. T., Momen, N., Drivdahl, S. B., & Simons, D. J. (2000). Change blindness blindness: The metacognitive error of overestimating change-detection ability. Visual Cognition, 7, 397–412.
Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Macmillan, N. A. (1986). The psychophysics of subliminal perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 38–39.
Marcel, A. J. (1983a). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238–300.
Marcel, A. J. (1983b). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237.
Marcel, A. J. (1998). Blindsight and shape perception: Deficit of visual consciousness or of visual function?Brain, 121(8), 1565–1588.
McCarthy, J. C., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 604–609.
McCormick, P. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23(1), 168–180.
McGlinchey-Berroth, R., Milberg, W. P., Verfaellie, M., Alexander, M., & Kilduff, P. T. (1993). Semantic processing in the neglected visual field: Evidence from a lexical decision task. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 79–108.
Merikle, P. (1992). Perception without awareness. Critical issues. American Psychologist, 47(6), 766–779.
Merikle, P. M. (1994). On the futility of attempting to demonstrate null awareness. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 17(3), 412.
Merikle, P., & Joordens, S. (1997a). Parallels between perception without attention and perception without awareness. Consciousness & Cognition, 6(2–3), 219–236.
Merikle, P. M., & Joordens, S. (1997b). Measuring unconscious influences. In J. W. Schooler (Ed.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 109–123). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Merikle, P. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1991). Comparing direct (explicit) and indirect (implicit) measures to study unconscious memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 224–233.
Merikle, P. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1992). Measuring unconscious perceptual processes. In T. S. Pittman (Ed.), Perception without awareness: Cognitive, clinical, and social perspectives (pp. 55–80). New York: Guilford Press.
Merikle, P., & Reingold, E. (1998). On demonstrating unconscious perception – Comment on Draine and Greenwald (1998). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(3), 304–310.
Merikle, P., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J. (2001). Perception without awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition, 79(1–2), 115–134.
Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mitroff, S. R., Simons, D. J., & Franconeri, S. L. (2002). The siren song of implicit change detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28(4), 798–815.
Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56–60.
Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., et al. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, 812–815.
Most, S. B., Simons, D. J., Scholl, B. J., Jimenez, R., Clifford, E., & Chabris, C. F. (2001). How not to be seen: The contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. Psychological Science, 12(1), 9–17.
Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424.
Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001a). The priming method: Imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. Cerebral Cortex, 11(10), 966–974.
Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001b). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80, 223–237.
Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct paramater specification? In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. ⅩⅤ, pp. 123–150). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pöppel, E., Held, R., & Frost, D. (1973). Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature, 243, 295–296.
Pratkanis, A. R. (1992). The cargo-cult science of subliminal persuasion. Skeptical Inquirer, 16, 260–272.
Pratkanis, A., Eskenazi, J., & Greenwald, A. (1994). What you expect is what you believe (but not necessarily what you get) – A test of the effectiveness of subliminal self-help audiotapes. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 15(3), 251–276.
Rees, G. (2001). Seeing is not perceiving. Nature Neuroscience, 4(7), 678–680.
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 261–270.
Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., & Driver, J. (2000). Unconscious activation of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a parietal patient with extinction. Brain, 123(8), 1624–1633.
Reingold, E., & Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 44(6), 563–757.
Rosa, M. G. P., Tweedale, R., & Elston, G. N. (2000). Visual responses of neurons in the middle temporal area of new world monkeys after lesions of striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(14), 5552–5563.
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.
Schacter, D. L., Alpert, N. M., Savage, C. R., Rauch, S. L., & Albert, M. S. (1996). Conscious recollection and the human hippocampal formation: Evidence from positron emission tomography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93(1), 321–325.
Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. (1998). On the relations among priming, conscious recollection, and intentional retrieval: Evidence from neuroimaging research. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 70(1–2), 284–303.
Schweinberger, S., & Stief, V. (2001). Implicit perception in patients with visual neglect: Lexical specificity in repetition priming. Neuropsychologia, 39(4), 420–429.
Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related markers of unconscious processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 209–218.
Sidis, B. (1898). The psychology of suggestion. New York: D. Appleton.
Simons, D. J. (2000). Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 147–155.
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.
Squire, L. R., Ojemann, J. G., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., Videen, T. O., & Raichle, M. E. (1992). Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: A functional anatomical study of memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 89(5), 1837–1841.
Stenberg, G., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Olsson, A., & Rosen, I. (2000). Semantic processing without conscious identification: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 26(4), 973–1004.
Teuber, H. L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1960). Visual field defects after penetrating missile wounds of the brain. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tong, F. (2001). Competing theories of binocular rivalry: A possible resolution. Brain and Mind, 2, 55–83.
Tong, F. (2003). Primary visual cortex and visual awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 219–229.
Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Vaughan, J. T., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). Binocular rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex. Neuron, 21, 753–759.
Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248.
Treisman, A. (1964). Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 449–459.
Trevethan, C. T., & Sahraie, A. (2003). Spatial and temporal processing in a subject with cortical blindness following occipital surgery. Neuropsychologia, 41(10), 1296–1306.
Verfaellie, M., Milberg, W. P., McGlinchey-Berroth, R., & Grande, L. (1995). Comparison of cross-field matching and forced-choice identification in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychology, 9, 427–434.
Visser, T., & Merikle, P. (1999). Conscious and unconscious processes: The effects of motivation. Consciousness & Cognition, 8(1), 94–113.
Volpe, B. T., Ledoux, J. E., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1979). Information processing of visual stimuli in an “extinguished” field. Nature, 282, 722–724.
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. (2002). Neural response to emotional faces with and without awareness: Event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2156–2166.
Vuilleumier, P., Sagiv, N., Hazeltine, E., Poldrack, R. A., Swick, D., Rafal, R. D., et al. (2001). Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: A combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(6), 3495–3500.
Watanabe, T., Nanez, J., & Sasaki, Y. (2001). Perceptual learning without perception. Nature, 413(6858), 844–848.
Weiskrantz, L. (2002). Prime-sight and blindsight. Consciousness & Cognition, 11(4), 568–581.
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., & Marshal, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97, 709–728.
Wessinger, C. M., Fendrich, R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1997). Islands of residual vision in hemianopic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(2), 203–221.
Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.
Wheatstone, C. (1838). Contributions to the physiology of vision – Part the first. On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 128, 371–394.
Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories: Cognition of brief visual stimuli (pp. 71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.