Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T01:26:17.816Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 6 - Enhanced Working Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2019

Michael W. Hoffmann
Affiliation:
University of Central Florida
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Assembly of the Executive Mind
Evolutionary Insights and a Paradigm for Brain Health
, pp. 107 - 123
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Maslin, MA, Shultz, S, Trauth, MH. A synthesis of the theories and concepts of early human evolution. Phil Trans R Soc B 2015 370: 20140064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, TJ, Uomini, NT, Rendell, LE, et al. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nat Commun 2015; 6: 6029. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. From mirror neurons to complex imitation in the evolution of language and tool use. Ann Rev Antropol 2011; 40: 257273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, RG. The Human Career, 3rd edn. University of Chicago Press, London, 2009.Google Scholar
Rowe, JB, Owen, AM, Johnsrude, IS, Passingham, RE. Imaging the mental components of a planning task. Neuropsychologia 2001; 39: 315327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bookstein, F, Schaefter, K, Prossinger, H, et al. Comparing frontal cranial profiles in archaic and modern Homo by morphometric analysis. Anat Record A Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol 1999; 257: 217224.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBrearty, S, Brooks, AS. The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol. 2000; 39, 453563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coolidge, FL, Wynn, T. The Rise of Homo Sapiens. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2018.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. Archeology and cognitive evolution. Behav Brain Sci 2002; 25: 389438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, M. Dissociable roles of the mid dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior inferotemporal cortex in visual working memory. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 74967503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champod, AS, Petrides, M. Dissociation within the frontoparietal network in verbal working memory: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 38493856.Google Scholar
Champod, AS, Petrides, M. Dissociable roles of the posterior parietal and the prefrontal cortex in manipulation and monitoring process. PNAS 2007; 104: 1483714842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mourre, V, Villa, P, Henshilwood, CS. Early use of pressure flaking on lithic artifacts at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Science. 2010; 330(6004): 659662.Google Scholar
Wynn, T, Coolidge, FI. The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition. Int J Evol Biol 2011. doi: 10.4061/2011/741357.Google Scholar
Wynn, T, Balter, M. Did working memory spark creative culture ? Science 2010; 328: 160163.Google Scholar
Eren, MI, Lycett, SJ. Why Levallois? A morphometric comparison of experimental “preferential” Levallois flakes versus debitage flakes. PLoS One 2012; 7(1): e29273. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Addis, DR, Wond, AT, Schacter, DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologica 2007; 45: 13631377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Land, MF. Do we have an internal model of the outside world? Philos Trans R Soc B 2014; 369: 20130045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, N. The Epigenetics Revolution. Columbia University Press, New York, 2012.Google Scholar
Testa-Silva, G, Verhoog, MB, Linaro, D, et al. High bandwidth synaptic communication and frequency tracking in human neocortex. PLoS Biol 2014; 12(11): e1002007. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squires, L. Gliotransmission. In: Brady, ST, Siegel, GJ, Albers, RW, Price, DL (eds.), Basic Neurochemistry, 8th edn. Elsevier Academic Press, New York 2012.Google Scholar
Coolidge, FL, Wynn, T. Cognitive prerequisites for a language of diplomacy. In: Tallerman, M, Gibson, KR (eds.), The Oxford Companion to Language Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2012.Google Scholar
Fuster, JM. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe. Lippincott-Raven, Philadelphia, PA, 1997.Google Scholar
Bruner, E. Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus: a neurofunctional perspective. Curr Anthropol 2010; 51: S77S88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeSmet, HJ, Paquir, P, Verhoeven, J, Mariën, P. The cerebellum: its role in language and related cognitive and affective functions. Brain Lang 2013; 127: 334342.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C, Frith, C, Malafouris, L. The Sapient Mind: Archeology Meets Neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, 2009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marean, C. When the sea saved humanity. Scientific American August 2010.Google Scholar
Wadley, L. Announcing a still bay industry at Sibudu Cave, South Africa. J Hum Evol 2007; 52: 681689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marean, CW. Pinnacle Point Cave 13B (Western Cape Province, South Africa) in context: the Cape Floral kingdom, shellfish, and modern human origins. J Hum Evol. 2010; 59(3–4): 425443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opperman, S. Out of Eden: The Peopling of the World. Robinson, London, 2004.Google Scholar
McBrearty, S. Palaeoanthropology: sharpening the mind. Nature. 2012; 491(7425): 531532.Google Scholar
Watson, E, Forster, P, Richards, M, Bandelt, HJ. Mitochondrial footprints of human expansions in Africa. Am J Human Genet 1997; 61: 691704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbons, A. Y chromosome shows that Adam was African. Science 1997; 278(5339): 804805.Google Scholar
Schlebusch, CM, Lombard, M, Soodyall, H. MtDNA control region variation affirms diversity and deep sub-structure in populations from southern Africa. BMC Evol Biol 2013; 13: 120.Google Scholar
Rito, T, Richards, MB, Fernandes, V, et al. The first modern human dispersals across Africa. PLoS One 2013; 8: e80031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tishkoff, SA, Gonder, MK, Henn, BM, et al. History of click speaking populations of Africa inferred from mtDNA and Y chromosome genetic variation. Mol Biol Evol 2007; 24: 21802195.Google Scholar
Pickrell, JK, Patterson, N, Barbieri, C, et al. The genetic prehistory of southern Africa. Nat Commun 2012. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henn, BM, Cavalli-Sforza, LL, Feldman, MW. The great human expansion. PNAS 2012; 109: 1775817764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, A, Underhill, PA, Mortensen, HM, et al. African Y chromosome and mtDNA divergence provides insight into the history of click languages. Curr Biol 2003; 13: 464473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, MA, Broadhurst, CL, Guest, M, et al. A quantum theory for the irreplaceable role of docosahexanoic acid in neural signaling throughout evolution. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 2013; 88: 513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, MA. Docosahexaenoic acid in neural signaling systems. Nutr Health 2006; 18(3): 263276.Google Scholar
Bazinet, RP, Laye, S. Polyunsaturated fatty acids and their metabolites in brain function and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 2014; 15: 771785.Google Scholar
Ramsden, CE, Faurot, KR, Zamora, D, et al. Targeted alteration of dietary n-3 and n-6 fatty acids for the treatment of chronic headaches: a randomized trial. Pain 2013; 154(11). doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.028.Google Scholar
Elston, GN. Pyramidal cells of the frontal lobe: all the more spinous to think with. J Neurosci 2000; 20: RC95.Google Scholar
Elston, GN. Cortex, cognition and the cell: new insights into the pyramidal neuron and prefrontal function. Cerebral Cortex 2003; 13: 11241138.Google Scholar
Bruner, E. Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 2004; 47: 279303.Google Scholar
Bruner, E, Manzi, G, Arsuaga, JL. Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: evidence from the Neanderthal and modern lineages. PNAS 2003; 100: 1533515340.Google Scholar
Bruner, E, De La Cuétara, JM, Holloway, R. A bivariate approach to the variation of the parietal curvature in the genus Homo. Anat Rec 2011; 294: 15481556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orban, GA, Caruana, F. The neural basis of human tool use. Front Psychol 2014; 5: 310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, E. The evolution of the parietal cortical areas in the human genus: between structure and cognition. In: Broadfield, D, Yuan, M, Schick, K, Toth, N (eds.), The Human Brain Evolving. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport, IN, 2010.Google Scholar
Overmann, KA. Finger counting in the upper Paleolithic. Rock Art Res 2014; 31: 6380.Google Scholar
Coolidge, FN, Overmann, KA. Numerosity, abstraction and the emergence of symbolic thinking. Curr Anthropol 2012; 53: 204225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavanna, AE, Trimble, MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 2006; 129: 564583.Google Scholar
Weaver, AH. Reciprocal evolution of the cerebellum and neocortex in fossil humans. PNAS 2005; 102: 35763580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandervert, LR, Schimpf, PH, Liu, H. How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation. Creat Res J 2007; 19: 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, RIM. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. J Hum Evol 1992; 20: 469493.Google Scholar
Barrett, L, Henzi, SP, Dunbar, RIM. Primate cognition: from “what now” to “what if?” Trends Cogn Sci 2003; 7: 494497.Google Scholar
Schultz, S, Nelson, E, Dunbar, RIM. Hominin cognitive evolution: identifying patterns and processes in the fossil and archaeological record. Phil Trans R Soc B 2012; 367: 21302140.Google Scholar
Richards, MP, Pettit, RB, Stiner, MC, Trinkaus, E. Stable isotope evidence for increasing dietary breadth in the European Mid-Upper Palaeolithic. PNAS 2001; 98: 65286532.Google Scholar
Pearce, E, Shuttleworth, A, Grove, M, Layton, RH. The costs of being a high-latitude hominin. In: Dunbar, RIM, Gamble, C, Gowlett, JAJ (eds.), Lucy to Language. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2014.Google Scholar
Grove, M. Amplitudes of orbitally induced climate cycles and patterns of hominin speciation. J Archaeol Sci 2012; 39: 30853094.Google Scholar
Pearce, E, Bridge, H. Is orbital volume associated with eyeball and visual cortical volume in humans ? Ann Hum Biol 2013; 40(6): 531540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balzeau, A, Holloway, RL, Grimaud-Herve, D. Variations and asymmetries in regional brain surface in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 2012; 62: 696706.Google Scholar
Pearce, E, Stringer, C, Dunbar, RIM. New insights into differences in brain organization between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans. Proc R Soc London B 2013; 280B. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0168.Google ScholarPubMed
McNabb, J. The importance of conveying visual information in Archeulean society: the background to the visual display hypothesis. Hum Evol 2011; 1: 123.Google Scholar
Dunbar, RIM, Gamble, C, Gowlett, JAJ. Lucy to Language. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S. The music instinct: the evolutionary basis of musicality. Ann NY Acad Sci 2009; 1169: 312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higham, T, Basell, L, Jacobi, R, et al. Τesting models for the beginnings of the Aurignacian and the advent of figurative art and music: the radiocarbon chronology of Geißenklösterle. J Hum Evol 2012; 62(6): 664676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conard, NJ, Bolus, M. Radiocarbon dating the appearance of modern humans and timing of cultural innovations in Europe: new results and new challenges. J Hum Evol 2003; 44(3): 331371.Google Scholar
Higham, T, Douka, K, Wood, R, et al. The timing and spatiotemporal patterning of Neanderthal disappearance. Nature 2014; 512: 306309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilpin, W, Feldman, MW, Aoki, K. An ecocultural model predicts Neanderthal extinction through competition with modern humans. PNAS 2016; 113(8): 21342139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×