Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T06:14:22.959Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Assisted imitation: affordances, effectivities, and the mirror system in early language development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2009

Patricia Zukow-Goldring
Affiliation:
University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA
Michael A. Arbib
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Rizzolatti and Arbib (1998) argue in their exposition of the Mirror System Hypothesis that brain mechanisms underlying human language abilities evolved from our non-human primate ancestors' ability to link self-generated actions and similar actions of others (see Arbib, Chapter 1, this volume). On this view, communicative gestures emerged eventually from a shared understanding that actions one makes oneself are indeed like those made by conspecifics. Thus, what the self knows can be enriched by an understanding of the actions and aims of others, and vice versa. From this perspective, the origins of language reside in behaviors not originally related to communication. That is, this common understanding of action sequences may provide a “missing link” to language.

In answering the question “What are the sources from outside the self that inform what the child knows?”, the basic idea is that negotiating a shared understanding of action grounds what individuals know in common, including foregrounding the body's part in detecting that the actions of the self are “like the other.” Given this footing, what then might the evolutionary path to language and the ontogeny of language in the child have in common? This perspective roots the source of the emergence of language in both as arising from perceiving and acting, leading to gesture, and eventually to speech.

I report here on an ongoing research program designed to investigate how perceiving and acting inform achieving a consensus or common understanding of ongoing events hypothesized to underlie communicating with language.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acredolo, L. P., and Goodwyn, S. W., 1985. Symbolic gesture in language development: a case study. Hum. Devel. 28: 40–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., 2002. The Mirror System, imitation, and evolution of language. In Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (eds.) Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge MA: MIT Press, pp. 229–280.Google Scholar
Atkeson, C. G., and Schaal, S., 1997. Robot learning from demonstration. Int. Conference on Machine Learning, San Francisco, CA, pp. 12–20.
Bahrick, L. E., and Pickens, J. N., 1994. Amodal relations: the basis for intermodal perception and learning in infancy. In Lewkowicz, D. J. and Lickliter, R. (eds.) The Development of Intersensory Perception: Comparative Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 205–233.Google Scholar
Bard, K. A., and Russell, C. L., 1999. Evolutionary foundations of imitation: social, cognitive and developmental aspects of imitative processes in non-human primates. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.) Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 89–123.Google Scholar
Bates, E., 1976. Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Begnini., L., Camaioni, L., Bretherton, I., and Volterra, V., 1979. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bicchi, A., 2000. Hands for dexterons manipulation and robust grasping: a difficult road towards simplicity. IEEE Trans. Robot. Automat. 16: 652–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billard, A., 2002. Imitation: a means to enhance learning of a synthetic proto-language in an autonomous robot. In Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (eds.) Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 281–310.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., 1993. Aspects of transmission of tool-use in wild chimpanzees. In Gibson, K. R. and Ingold, T. (eds.) Tools, Language, Cognition in Human Evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 171–183.Google Scholar
Bråten, S., 2002. Altercentric perception by infants and adults in dialogue. In Stamenov, M. I. and Gallese, V. (eds.) Mirror Neurons and the Evolution of Brain and Language. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Benjamin, pp. 273–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braunwald, S., 1978. Context, word and meaning: toward a communicational analysis of lexical acquisition. In Lock, A. (ed.) Action, Gesture, and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. London: Academic Press, pp. 285–327.Google Scholar
Braunwald, S., 1979. On being understood: the listener's contribution to the toddler's ability to communicate. In French, P. (ed.) The Development of Meaning. Hiroshima, Japan: Bunko Hyonron Press, pp. 71–113.Google Scholar
Breazeal, C., and Scassellati, B., 2002. Robots that imitate humans. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6: 481–487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bril, B., 2004. Learning to use scissors: adult's scaffolding and task properties. Proceedings Int. Congress of Psychology, Beijing, China, pp. 545–546.
Bruner, J., 1983. Children's Talk: Learning to Use Language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Butcher, C., and Goldin-Meadow, S., 2000. Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: when hand and mouth come together. In McNeill, D. (ed.) Language and Gesture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 235–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., 2003. Imitation as behaviour parsing. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. London B 358: 529–536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E., and Volterra, V., 1996. Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. J. Child Lang. 23: 645–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., 1993. The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costall, A., 1989. A closer look at “direct perception.” In Gellatly, A., Rodgers, D., and Sloboda, J. A. (eds.) Cognition and Social Worlds.Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press, pp. 10–21.Google Scholar
Costall, A., 1995. Socializing affordances. Theory Psychol. 5: 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dautenhahn, K., and Nehaniv, C. L., 2002. The agent-based perspective on imitation. In Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (eds.) Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–40.Google Scholar
Eckerman, C. O., 1993. Toddlers' achievement of coordinated action with conspecifics: a dynamic systems perspective. In Smith, L. B. and Thelen, E. (eds.) A Dynamic Systems Approach to Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 333–357.Google Scholar
Falk, D., 2004. Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behav. Brain Sci. 27: 491–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., et al., 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Devel. 59.Google ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. J., 1969. Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J., 2002. Perceiving the Affordances: A Portrait of Two Psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J., and Pick, A. D., 2000. An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J., and Rader, N., 1979. Attention: the perceiver as performer. In Hale, G. A. and Lewis, M. (eds.) Attention and Cognitive Development. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J., 1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J., and Pick, A. D., 1963. Perception of another person's looking behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 76: 386–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, K. R., 1993. Introduction: generative interplay between technical capacities, social relations, imitation and cognition. In Gibson, K. R. and Ingold, T. (eds.) Tools, Language, Cognition in Human Evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 131–137.Google Scholar
Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E., and Watson, J. D., 2000. A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Devel. 71: 878–894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogate, L. J., Walker-Andrews, A. S., and Bahrick, L. E., 2001. The intersensory origins of word comprehension: an ecological–dynamic systems view. Devel. Sci. 4: 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R., Mervis, C., and Hirsh-Pasek, K., 1994. Early object labels: the case for a developmental lexical principles framework. J. Child Lang. 21: 125–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, C. (ed.), 2000. Vision and inscription in practice. Mind, Culture, and Activity 7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., 1972. Cross-cultural studies of mother–infant interaction: toward a structural–functional approach. Hum. Devel. 15: 131–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., and Smith, J., 1976. The Structure of Communication in Early Language Development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Reilly, J., Leaper, C., and Baker, N., 1985. The structural and functional status of single-word utterances. In Barrett, M. D. (ed.) Children's Single-Word Speech. Chichester, UK: John Wiley, pp. 233–267.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Maynard, A. E., Boehm, C., and Schmidtling, E. Y., 2000. Cultural apprenticeship and cultural change: tool learning and imitation in chimpanzees and humans. In Parker, S. T., Langer, J., and McKinney, M. L. (eds.) Biology, Brains and Behaviour. Santa Fe, NM: SAR Press, pp. 237–277.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M., 2001. Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?Anim. Behav. 61: 139–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heft, H., 1989. Affordances and the body: an intentional analysis of Gibson's ecological approach to visual perception. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 19: 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., et al., 1999. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286: 2526–2528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingold, T., 2000. The Perception of the Environment: Essays in Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., and Caselli, M. C., 1994. From communication to language in two modalities. Cogn. Devel. 9: 23–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, M. I., and Rumelhart, D. E., 1992. Forward models: supervised learning with a distal teacher. Cogn. Sci. 16: 307–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kadar, E., and Shaw, R. E., 2000. Toward an ecological field theory of perceptual control of locomotion. Ecol. Psychol. 12: 141–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A., 2004. Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macbeth, D., 1994. Classroom encounters with the unspeakable: “Do you see, Danelle?”Discourse Proc. 17: 311–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mace, W., 1977. Ask not what's in your head, but what your head's inside of. In Shaw, R. E. and Bransford, J. (eds.) Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 43–65.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M., 1989. Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Martini, M., and Kirkpatrick, J., 1992. Parenting in Polynesia: a view from the Marquesas. In Roopnarine, J. L. and Carter, D. B. (eds.) Parent–Child Socialization in Diverse Cultures. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 199–223.Google Scholar
Maynard, A. E., 2002. Cultural teaching: the development of teaching skills in Zinacantec Maya sibling interactions. Child Devel. 73: 969–982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGovern, A., and Barto, A. G., 2001. Automatic discovery of subgoals in reinforcement learning using diverse density. Proceedings 18th Int. Conference on Machine Learning, San Francisco, CA, pp. 361–368.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K., 1995. Infants' understanding of people and things: from body imitation to folk psychology. In Bermúdez, J., Marcel, A. J., and Eilan, N. (eds.) The Body and the Self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 43–69.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K., 1999. Persons and representation: why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.) Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 9–35.Google Scholar
Michaels, C. F., and Carello, C., 1981. Direct Perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Moerman, M., 1988. Talking Culture: Ethnography and Conversation Analysis. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., 2005. Discussion: Imitation, Identification, and Self–Other Awareness. Atlanta, GA: Society for Research on Child Development.Google Scholar
Moore, C., and Dunham, P. (eds), 1995. Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), 1999. Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Guérini, C., Pezé, A., and Rivet, C., 1999. The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.) Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 209–234.Google Scholar
Nehaniv, C. L., and Dautenhahn, K., 2002. The correspondence problem. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.) Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 41–61.Google Scholar
Oztop, E., and Arbib, M. A., 2002. Schema design and implementation of the grasp-related mirror neuron system. Biol. Cybernet. 87: 116–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oztop, E., Bradley, N. S., and Arbib, M. A., 2004. Infant grasp learning: a computational model. Exp. Brain Res. 158: 480–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J., 1962. Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Bering, J. M., and Giambrone, S., 2000. Toward a science of other minds: escaping the argument by analogy. Cogn. Sci. 24: 509–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quiatt, D., and Itani, J., 1994. Hominid Culture in Primate Perspective. Niwot, CO: University Press of Colorado.Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. O., 1960. Word and Object. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Reboul, A., 2004. Evolution of language from theory of mind or coevolution of language and theory of mind? Available at http://www.interdisciplines
Reed, E. S., 1995. The ecological approach to language development. Lang. Communi. 15: 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., and Arbib, M. A., 1998. Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci. 21: 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., 1986. Ape Language: From Conditioned Response to Symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., and Lewin, R., 1994. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Schaall, S., 1999. Is imitation learning the route to humanoid robots? Trends Cogn. Sci. 3: 233–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M., 1982. Steps to Language: Toward a Theory of Native Language Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schutz, A., 1962. Collected Papers, vol. 1, The Problem of Social Reality. Leiden, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.
Sharrock, W., and Coulter, J., 1998. On what we can see. Theory Psychol. 8: 147–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R., 2001. Processes, acts, and experiences: three stances on the problem of intentionality. Ecol. Psychol. 13: 275–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R., and Turvey, M., 1981. Coalitions as models of ecosystems: a realist perspective on perceptual organization. In Kubovy, M. and Pomerantz, J. R. (eds.) Perceptual Organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 343–415.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Perlmann, R., and Nathan, D., 1987. Why routines are different: toward a multiple-factor model of the relation between input and language acquisition. In Nelson, K. E. and Kleeck, A. (eds.) Children's Language, vol. 6. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 65–97.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., 1979. Perceiving bimodally specified events in infancy. Devel. Psychol. 15: 626–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K., 2002. Primate worlds. In Heyes, C. and Huber, L. (eds.) The Evolution of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 143–162.Google Scholar
Stoffregen, T. A., 2003. Affordances as properties of the animal–environment system. Ecol. Psychol. 15: 115–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoffregen, T., and Bardy, B., 2001. On specification and the senses. Behav. Brain Sci. 24: 195–261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoffregen, T. A., Gorday, K. M., Sheng, Y., and Flynn, S. B., 1999. Perceiving affordances for another person's actions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 25: 120–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., and Smith, L., 1994. A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L., 1898. Animal intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychol. Rev. Monogr.2.
Tomasello, M., 1988. The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Lang. Sci. 10: 69–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., 2001. Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life. In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.) Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 132–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Call, J., 1997. Primate Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., and Ratner, H. H., 1993. Cultural learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 16: 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and Kruger, A. C., 1993. Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Devel. 64: 1688–1705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turvey, M., 1992. Affordances and prospective control: an outline of the ontology. Ecol. Psychol. 4: 173–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M., Shaw, R., Reed, E., and Mace, W., 1981. Ecological laws of perceiving and acting. Cognition 9: 237–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uzgiris, I., 1991. The social context of imitation. In Lewis, M. and Feinman, S. (eds.) Social Influences and Socialization in Infancy. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 215–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visalberghi, E., and Fragaszy, D., 2002. “Do monkeys ape?” – ten years later. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.) Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 471–499.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., and Pizzuto, E., 2005. Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In Tomasello, M. and Slobin, D. (eds.) Beyond Nature–Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 3–40.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S., 1978. Mind in Society.Cole, (M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., and Souberman, E., eds.) Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Watson-Gegeo, K. A., and Gegeo, D. W., 1986. Calling-out and repeating routines in Kwara'ae children's language socialization. In B. Schieffelin, and E. Ochs (eds.) Language Socialization across Cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 17–50.Google Scholar
Watson-Gegeo, K. A., and Gegeo, D. W., 1999. (Re)modeling culture in Kwara'ae: the role of discourse in children's cognitive development. Discourse Stud. 1: 241–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. V., McNamee, G. D., McLane, J. B., and Budwig, N., 1980. The adult–child dyad as a problem-solving system. Child Devel. 51: 1215–1221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., 2002. Imitation of sequential and hierarchical structure in action: experimental studies with children and chimpanzees. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.) Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 192–209.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., and Ham, R., 1992. On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. In Slater, P. J. B., Rosenblatt, J. S., Beer, C., and Milinski, M. (eds.) Advances in the Study of Behavior, vol. 21. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 239–283.Google Scholar
Witherington, D., 2005. A comparison of two conceptualizations for the concept of affordance. Proceedings Int. Conference on Perception and Action, Asilomar, CA.
Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., and Ross, G., 1976. The role of tutoring in problem solving. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 17: 89–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zukow, P. G., 1989. Siblings as effective socializing agents: evidence from Central Mexico. In Zukow, P. G. (ed.) Sibling Interactions across Cultures: Theoretical and Methodological Issues. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 79–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zukow, P. G., 1990. Socio-perceptual bases for the emergence of language: an alternative to innatist approaches. Devel. Psychobiol. 23: 705–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zukow, P. G., 1996. Sensitive caregivers foster the comprehension of speech: when gestures speak louder than words. Early Devel. Parent. 5: 195–211.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zukow, P. G., 1997. A social ecological realist approach to the emergence of the lexicon: educating attention to amodal invariants in gesture and speech. In Dent-Read, C. and Zukow-Goldring, P. (eds.) Evolving Explanations of Development: Ecological Approaches to Organism-Environment Systems. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 199–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zukow, P. G., 2001. Perceiving referring actions: Latino and Euro-American infants and caregivers comprehending speech. In Nelson, K. E., Aksu-Koc, A., and Johnson, C. (eds.) Children's Language, vol. 11. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 139–163.Google Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P., and Arbib, M. A., in press. Affordances, effectivities, and assisted imitation: caregivers and the direction of attention. Neurocomp.
Zukow-Goldring, P., and Rader, N., 2001. Perceiving referring actions: a commentary on Gogate, Walker-Andrews, and Bahrick. Devel. Sci. 4: 28–30.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×