We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
A summary is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
Akaike, H., 1973. Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle, in Second International Symposium on Information Theory, eds. Petrov, B. N. & Csaki, F.. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado, 267–81.Google Scholar
Arnold, C., Matthews, L. J. & Nunn, C. L., 2010. The 10k Trees website: a new online resource for primate phylogeny. Evolutionary Anthropology19, 114–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auerbach, J. D., Kanarek, A. & Burghardt, G. M., 2015. To play or not to play? That’s a resource abundance question. Adaptive Behavior 23, 354–61.
Baldwin, J. M., 1895. Mental Development in the Child and the Race. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M., 1896. A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist30, 441–51, 536–53.Google Scholar
Batnitzky, L., 2011. How Judaism Became a Religion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P., 2011. Theories of play, in The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play, ed. Pellegrini, A. D.. New York: Oxford University Press, 41–7.Google Scholar
Beaulieu, J. M., Jhwueng, D.-C., Boettiger, C. & O’Meara, B. C., 2012. Modeling stabilizing selection: expanding the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck model of adaptive evolution. Evolution 66, 2369–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, C.2009b, org. 1997. Ritual. Perspectives and Dimensions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bellah, R. N., 2011. Religion in Human Evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M. & Pierce, J., 2009. Wild Justice: The Moral Lives of Animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bering, J., 2011. The Belief Instinct. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Bininda-Emonds, O. R. P., Cardillo, M., Jones, K. E., MacPhee, R. D. E., Beck, R. M. D., Grenyer, R., Price, S. A., Vos, R. A., Gittleman, J. L. & Purvis, A., 2007. The delayed rise of present-day mammals. Nature446, 507–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P., 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Boyer, P. & Liénard, P., 2006. Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological, and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences29, 1–56 (includes commentaries and responses).Google ScholarPubMed
Bulbia, J., Sosis, R., Harris, E., Genet, R., Genet, C. & Wyman, K. (eds.), 2008. The Evolution of Religion: Studies, Theories, & Critiques. Santa Margarita, CA: Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 1973. Instinct and innate behavior: toward an ethological psychology, in The Study of Behavior: Learning, Motivation, Emotion, and Instinct, eds. Nevin, J. A. & Reynolds, G. S.. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman, 322–400.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 1988. Precocity, play, and the ectotherm-endotherm transition: profound reorganization or superficial adaptation, in Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 9, Developmental Psychobiology and Behavioral Ecology, ed. Blass, E. M.. New York: Plenum, 107–48.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2010. The comparative reach of play and brain: perspective, evidence, and implications. American Journal of Play2, 338–56.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2011. Defining and recognizing play, in The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play, ed. Pellegrini, A. D.. New York: Oxford University Press, 9–18.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2013a. Play, animals, resources: the need for a rich (and challenging) comparative environment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences36, 5, 20–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burghardt, G. M., 2013b. The Janus faced nature of comparative psychology – strength or weakness?Evolutionary Psychology11, 762–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burghardt, G. M., 2014a. A brief glimpse at the long evolutionary history of play. Animal Behavior and Cognition1, 90–8.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2015. Creativity, play, and the pace of evolution, in Animal Creativity and Innovation, eds. Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C.. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier, 129–59.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M. & Bowers, R. I., 2017. From instinct to behavior systems: an integrated approach to ethological psychology, in APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Vol 1. Basic Concepts, Methods, Neural Substrate, and Behavior, eds. J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 333–64.
Burghardt, G. M., Murphy, J. B., Chiszar, D. & Hutchins, M., 2009. Combating ophiophobia: origins, treatment, education and conservation tools. In Snakes: Ecology and Conservation, eds. Mullin, S. & Seigel, R.. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 262–80.Google Scholar
Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R., 2002. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach. Berlin: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., 2011. Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind, 4th ed. Boston, MA: Pearson, Allyn, and Bacon.Google Scholar
Butler, M. A. & King, A. A., 2004. Phylogenetic comparative analysis: a modeling approach for adaptive evolution. American Naturalist164, 683–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Braak, H. van. 2013. Evolutionary Psychology. Harlow: Pearson.Google Scholar
Charlesworth, J. H., 2010. The Good and Evil Serpent. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I., 1999. Grundiss der Vergleichennden Verhaltensforschung. Munich: Piper.Google Scholar
Eilam, D., Zor, R., Szechtman, H. & Hermesh, H., 2006. Rituals, stereotypy, and compulsive behavior in animals and humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews30, 456–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernest, S. K. M., 2003. Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mammals. Ecology84, 3402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feierman, J. R. (ed.), 2009. The Biology of Religious Behavior: The Evolutionary Origins of Faith and Religion. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.Google Scholar
Felsenstein, J., 1985. Phylogenies and the comparative method. American Naturalist125, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FitzJohn, R. G., Maddison, W. P. & Otto, S. P., 2009. Estimating trait-dependent speciation and extinction rates from incompletely resolved phylogenies. Systematic Biology58, 595–611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, S. A., 1995. Constraint, adaptation, and opportunism in the design of behavioral phenotypes. In Perspectives in Ethology: Behavioral Design (Volume 11, ed. Thompson, N. S.). New York: Plenum Press, 61–81.Google Scholar
Frazer, J. G., 1890. The Golden Bough: A Study in Comparative Religion. 2 vol. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Graham, K. L. & Burghardt, G. M., 2010. Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress. Quarterly Review of Biology85, 393–418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groos, K., 1898. The Play of Animals (E. L. Baldwin, trans.). New York: Appleton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S., 1954. Introducing Douglas Spalding. British Journal of Animal Behaviour2, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon, L. J., Weir, J. T., Brock, C. D., Glor, R. E. & Challenger, W., 2008. GEIGER: investigating evolutionary radiations. Bioinformatics24, 129–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinroth, O., 1911. Beiträge zur Biologie, namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Deutsche Ornithologische Gesellshaft 1910, 589–702.Google Scholar
Henricks, T. S.2015. Play and the Human Condition. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Hood, R. W., Jr. & Williamson, W. P., 2008. Them that Believe: The Power and Meaning of the Christian Serpent-Handling Tradition. Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huffman, M. A., 1984. Stone-play of Macaca fuscata in Arashiyama B troop: transmission of a non-adaptive behaviour. Journal of Human Evolution13, 725–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huizinga, J., 1955. Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play Element in Culture. Boston, MA: Beacon.Google Scholar
Hurd, W., 1785. A New Universal History of the Religious Rites, Ceremonies, and Customs of the Whole World: Or, a Complete and Impartial View of all the Religions in the Various Nations of the Universe. London: Alexander Hogg.Google Scholar
Huxley, J. S., 1914. The courtship habits of the Great Crested Grebe (Podiceps cristatus); with an addition to the theory of sexual selection. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London35, 491–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lande, R., 2009. Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation. Journal of Evolutionary Biology22, 1435–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, A., 1899. Myth, Ritual and Religion. 2 vol., new edition. London: Longmans, Green, and Co.Google Scholar
Lehner, P. N.1996. Handbook of Ethological Methods, 2nd Ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Liebel, K. & Call, J., 2012. The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B367, 118–28.Google Scholar
Liénard, P. & Boyer, P., 2006. Whence collective rituals? A cultural selection model of ritualized behaviour. American Anthropologist108, 814–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, H. R., 1898. Instinct and Reason. London: Macmillan Co.Google Scholar
Matthews, J., 2011. Starting from Scratch: The Origin and Development of Expression, Representation and Symbolism in Human and Non-human Primates. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
McCauley, R. N., 2011. Why Religion Is Natural and Science Is Not. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, D. L., 1970. Gods and Games: Toward a Theology of Play. New York: World Publishing.Google Scholar
Miller, J. H. & Page, S. E., 2007. Complex Adaptive Systems: An Introduction to Computational Models of Social Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Moczek, A. P., Sultan, S., Foster, S., Ledón-Rettig, C., Dworkin, I., Nijhout, H. F., Abouheit, E. & Pfennig, D. W., 2011. The role of developmental plasticity in evolutionary innovation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B278 (1719), 2705–13.Google Scholar
Nunn, C. L., Altizer, S., Sechrest, W., Jones, K. E., Barton, R. A. & Gittleman, J. L., 2004. Parasites and the evolutionary diversification of primate clades. American Naturalist164, S90–S103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Meara, B. C., Ane, C., Sanderson, M. J. & Wainwright, P. C., 2006. Testing for different rates of continuous trait evolution using likelihood. Evolution60, 922–33.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Meara, B. C., Graham, K. L., Pellis, S. M. & Burghardt, G. M., 2015. Evolutionary models for the retention of adult–adult social play in primates: the roles of diet and other factors associated with resource acquisition. Adaptive Behavior 23, 381–91.
Pagel, M., 1994. Detecting correlated evolution on phylogenies – a general-method for the comparative-analysis of discrete characters. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences255, 37–45.Google Scholar
Pagel, M., 1997. Inferring evolutionary processes from phylogenies. Zoologica Scripta26, 331–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Burghardt, G. M., Smuts, B., Cordoni, G., Dall’Olio, S., Fouts, H. N., Reháková-Petr˚u, M., Siviy, S. M. & Pellis, S. M., 2016. Rough-and-tumble play as a window on animal communication. Biological Reviews91, 111–27. doi: 10.1111/brv.12172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J., 1998. Affective Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Biven, L., 2012. The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pannenberg, W., 1985. Anthropology in Theological Perspective. Philadelphia, PA. Westminster Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., 2009. The Role of Play in Human Development. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, A.D. (ed.), 2011. The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pellis, S. M. & Iwaniuk, A. N., 2000. Adult–adult play in primates: comparative analyses of its origin, distribution and evolution. Ethology106, 1083–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellis, S. M. & Pellis, V. C., 2009. The Playful Brain. Venturing to the Limits of Neuroscience. Oxford: Oneworld Press.Google Scholar
Pruitt, J. N., Burghardt, G. M & Riechert, S. E., 2012. Nonconceptive sexual behaviour in spiders: a form of play associated with body condition, personality type, and male intrasexual selection. Ethology118, 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyysiäinen, I., 2009. Supernatural Agents. Why We Believe in Souls, Gods, and Buddhas. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team, 2012. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Riis, O. & Woodhead, L., 2012. A Sociology of Religious Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, L. J. & Kaplan, G., 2002. Songs, Roars, and Rituals. Communication in Birds, Mammals, and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A., 1675. Panzebia or a View of all the Religions in the World (5th ed.).London: John Williams.Google Scholar
Rossano, M. J., 2010. Supernatural Selection. How Religion Evolved. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, M. J.2012. The essential role of ritual in the transmission and reinforcement of social norms. Psychological Bulletin 138, 129–49.
Russell, N. & McGowan, K. J., 2002. Dances of the cranes: crane symbolism at Catalhöyük and beyond. Antiquities77 (297), 445–55.Google Scholar
Sax, W. S., 1995. The Gods at Play. Līlā in South Asia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sax, W. S., Quack, J. & Weinhold, J. (eds.), 2010. The Problem of Ritual Efficacy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachner, A., Brady, T. F., Pepperberg, I. M. & Hauser, M. D., 2009. Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species. Current Biology19, 831–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schechner, R.2013. Performance Studies: An Introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Seligman, A. B., Weller, R. P., Puett, M. J. & Simon, B., 2008. Ritual and its Consequences: An Essay on the Limits of Sincerity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stack, J. C., Harmon, L. J & O’Meara, B. C., 2011. RBrownie: an R package for testing hypotheses about rates of evolutionary change. Methods in Ecology and Evolution2, 660–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, G. N., Nee, S. & Felsenstein, J. N., 2011. Controlling for non-independence in comparative analysis of patterns across populations within species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B – Biological Sciences366, 1410–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton-Smith, B., 1997. The Ambiguity of Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Reamer, L., Schapiro, S. J. & Hopkins, W. D., 2012. Social learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees. Biology Letters8, 498–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorpe, W. H., 1966. Ritualization in ontogeny: I. Animal play. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B – Biological Sciences251 (772), 311–19.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J., Frost, G. Y., Carpenter, M. & Nagell, K., 1997. The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: a comparison across groups and generations. Evolution of Communication1, 223–53.Google Scholar
Turner, V., 1982. From Ritual to Theatre: The Human Seriousness of Play. New York: PAJ Publications.Google Scholar
Van de Vliert, E., 2013. Climato-economic habitats support patterns of human needs, stresses, and freedoms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences36 (5), 1–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, N., 2009. The Faith Instinct. How Religion Evolved and Why it Matters. New York: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C., 2001. Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour138, 1481–516.Google Scholar
References
Auersperg, A. M. I., von Bayern, A. M. P., Gajdon, G. K., Huber, L. & Kacelnik, A.2011. Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi access box paradigm. PLoSone6, e20231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auger, A. P. & Olesen, K. M.2009. Brain sex differences and the organisation of juvenile social play behaviour. Journal of Neuroendocrinology21, 519–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, P.1981. Discontinuities in development and changes in the organization of play in cats, eds Immelmann, K., Barlow, G. W., Petrinovich, L. & Main, M., Behavioral Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 281–95.Google Scholar
Bateson, P.2011. Theories of play, ed. Pellegrini, A. D.. The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play. New York:Oxford University Press, 41–7.Google Scholar
Bateson, P.2014. Play, playfulness, creativity and innovation. Animal Behavior and Cognition1, 99–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. & Martin, P.2013. Play, Playfulness, Creativity and Innovation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. & Nettle, D.2014. Playfulness, ideas and creativity: a survey. Creativity Research Journal26, 219–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birch, H. G.1945. The relation of previous experience to insightful problem-solving. Journal of Comparative Psychology38, 367–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, C. D. & Emery, N. J.2009. Rooks use stones to raise the water level to reach a floating worm. Current Biology19, 1410–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burghardt, G. M.2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, E. Z., Linklater, W. L., Stafford, K. J. & Minot, E. O.2008. Maternal investment results in better foal condition through increased play behaviour in horses. Animal Behaviour76, 1511–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheke, L. D., Bird, C. D. & Clayton, N. S.2011. Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius). Animal Cognition14, 441–55.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M.1996. Creativity: Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Einon, D. & Potegal, M.1991. Enhanced defense in adult-rats deprived of playfighting experience as juveniles. Aggressive Behavior17, 27–40.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagen, R.1981. Animal Play Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. & Fagen, J.2004. Juvenile survival and benefits of play behaviour in brown bears, Ursus arctos. Evolutionary Ecology Research6, 89–102.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. & Fagen, J.2009. Play behaviour and multi-year juvenile survival in free-ranging brown bears, Ursus arctos. Evolutionary Ecology Research11, 1–15.Google Scholar
Fertl, D. & Wilson, B.1997. Bubble use during prey capture by a lone bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Aquatic Mammals23, 113–14.Google Scholar
Gomendio, M.1988. The development of different types of play in gazelles: implications for the nature and functions of play. Animal Behaviour36, 825–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graves, P. L.1976. Nutrition, infant behavior, and maternal characteristics: a pilot study in West Bengal, India. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition29, 305–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harcourt, R.1991. Survivorship costs of play in the South American fur seal. Animal Behaviour42, 509–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausberger, M., Fureix, C., Bourjade, M., Wessel-Robert, S. & Richard-Yris, M.-A.2012. On the significance of adult play: what does social play tell us about adult horse welfare?Naturwissenschaften99, 291–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, S. D. E. & Spinka, M.2011. Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour81, 891–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, L., Rechberger, S. & Taborsky, M.2001. Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. Animal Behaviour62, 945–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, A. P. & Einon, D. F.1981. Play as a reinforcer for maze-learning in juvenile rats. Animal Behaviour29, 259–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. R.1996. Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature379, 249–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, M. B.1999. Effects of confinement on rebounds of locomotor behaviour of calves and heifers, and the spatial preferences of calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science62, 43–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M. & Wrangham, R. W.2010. Sex differences in chimpanzees’ use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology20, R1067–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köhler, W.1925. The Mentality of Apes. London: Paul, Trench & Trubner.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Makecha, R., Trone, M., Paulos, R. D. & Ramos, J. A. A.2006. Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International Journal of Comparative Psychology19, 223–40.Google Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. & Diener, E.2005. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success?Psychological Bulletin131, 803–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manning, A. & Dawkins, M. S.2012. An Introduction to Animal Behaviour. 6th edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, P. & Caro, T. M.1985. On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. Advances in the Study of Behavior15, 59–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaney, M. J. & Stewart, J.1985. Sex differences in social play: the socialization of sex roles. Advances in the Study of Behavior15, 1–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M.1988. The effects of litter-size variation on the development of play-behaviour in the domestic cat – litters of one and two. Animal Behaviour36, 20–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milteer, R. M. & Ginsburg, K. R.2012. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent–child bond: focus on children in poverty. Pediatrics129, e204–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D.2007. Personality: What Makes You the Way You Are. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J.1998. Affective Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D.2009. The Role of Play in Human Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, T. G.2000. Play and Exploration in Children and Animals. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pruitt, J. N., Burghardt, G. M. & Riechert, S. E.2012. Non-conceptive sexual behavior in spiders: a form of play associated with body condition, personality type, and male intrasexual selection. Ethology118, 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pryor, K. W., Haag, R. & O’Reilly, J.1969. The creative porpoise: training for novel behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior12, 653–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sessa, B.2008. Is it time to revisit the role of psychedelic drugs in enhancing human creativity?Journal of Psychopharmacology22, 821–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, L. L., Clutton-Brock, T. H., Brotherton, P. N. M., Cameron, E. Z. & Cherry, M. I.2002. Experimental provisioning increases play in free-ranging meerkats. Animal Behaviour64, 113–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J.2010. Cognition, Evolution and Behavior 2nd edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Neumann, C., Baksh, M., Bwibo, N. & McDonald, M. A.1989. Relationship between nutrition and development in Kenyan toddlers. Journal of Pediatrics115, 357–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, P. K.2010. Children and Play. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sutton-Smith, B.1997. The Ambiguity of Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P.1972. Predictive validity of Torrance tests of creative thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior6, 236–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J.1968. The Double Helix: A Personal Account of the Discovery of the Structure of DNA. New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
West, M.1974. Social play in the cat. American Zoologist14, 427–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiley, D., Ware, C., Bocconcelli, A., Cholewiak, D., Friedlaender, A., Thompson, M. et al. 2011. Underwater components of humpback whale bubble-net feeding behaviour. Behaviour148, 575–602.Google Scholar
References
Alexander, R. D.1989. Evolution of the human psyche, in The Human Revolution, eds. Mellars, P. & Stringer, C.. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, 455–513.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. & Allen, C.1998. Intentional communication and social play: how and why animals negotiate and agree to play, in Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives, eds. Bekoff, M. & Byers, J. A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 97–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blurton Jones, N. G.1993. The lives of hunter-gatherer children: Effects of parental behavior and parental reproductive strategy, in Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behavior, eds. M. E. Pereira and L. A. Fairbanks. New York: Oxford University Press, 309–25.
Breuggeman, J. A.1978. The function of adult play in free-ranging Macaca mulatto, in Social Play in Primates, ed. Smith, E. O.. New York: Academic Press, 169–91.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S.1986. Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P.2002. Human creativity: its cognitive basis, its evolution, and its connection with childhood pretence. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 53, 225–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christie, J. F. & Roskos, K. A.2006. Standards, science, and the role of play in early literacy education, in Play=Learning, eds. Singer, D., Golinkoff, R. & Hirsh-Pasek, K.. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 57–73.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M.1989. Peacemaking Among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Engel, S.2005. The narrative worlds of what is and what if. Cognitive Development, 20, 514–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fein, G. G.1975. A transformational analysis of pretending. Developmental Psychology, 11, 291–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B. & Zelazo, P.1972. The developmental progression of manipulative play in the first two years. Child Development, 47, 232–5.Google Scholar
Gaskins, S.1999. Children’s lives in a Mayan village: a case of culturally constructed roles and activities, in Children’s Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives, ed. Göncü, A.. New York: Cambridge University Press, 25–61.Google Scholar
Gaskins, S., Haight, W. & Lancy, D. F.2007. The cultural construction of play, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 179–202.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. & Martín-Andrade, B.2005. Fantasy play in apes, in The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans, eds. Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K.. New York: Guilford, 139–72.Google Scholar
Göncü, , Patt, A., , M. B. & Kouba, E.2002. Understanding young children’s pretend play in context, in Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development, eds. Smith, P. K. & Hart, C. H.. Oxford: Blackwell, 418–37.Google Scholar
Göncü, A. & Perone, A.2005. Pretend play as a life-span activity. Topoi, 24, 137–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosso, Y., Otta, E., Morais, M. L. S., Ribeiro, F. J. L. & Bussab, V. S. R.2005. Play in hunter-gatherer society, in The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans, eds. Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K.. New York: Guilford, 213–53.Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M.1983. How children become friends. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48 no 3; serial no 201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haight, W. L. & Miller, P. J.1993. Pretending at Home: Early Development in a Sociocultural Context. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L.1994. Understanding pretense, in Children’s Early Understanding of Mind, eds. Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P.. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum, 235–9.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L.2000. The Work of the Imagination. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L.2007. Hard work for the imagination, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 205–25.Google Scholar
Hayes, C. H.1951. The Ape in our House. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Holland, P.2003. We Don’t Play with Guns here. Philadelphia. PA: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Howes, C. & Matheson, C. C.1992. Sequences in the development of competent play with peers. Social and social pretend play. Developmental Psychology, 28, 961–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C.2003. A review of research into pretend play in autism. Autism, 7, 379–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Carruthers, P., Smith, P. K. & Boucher, J.1994. Pretend play: is it metarepresentational?Mind and Language, 9, 445–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanlenberg, S. M. & Wrangham, R. W.2010. Sex differences in chimpanzees' use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology, 20, R1067–8.Google Scholar
Kelly, R.2006. An Exploration of the Role of Executive Functions in the Symbolic Play of Children with High-Functioning Autism, Children with Asperger’s Disorder, and Typically Developing Children. Unpublished doctoral thesis, LaTrobe University, Australia.
Konner, M.1972. Aspects of the developmental ethology of a forging people, in Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour, ed. Jones, N. Blurton. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 285–304.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F.1996. Playing on the Mother Ground: Cultural Routines for Children’s Development. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S.1994. Making sense of pretence, in Children’s Early Understanding of Mind, eds. Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P.. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum, 211–34.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D. & Palmquist, C. M.2013. The impact of pretend play on children’s development: a review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.2005. Language evolution and human development, in Origins of the Social Mind, eds. Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F.. New York & London: Guilford Press, 383–410.Google Scholar
Martini, M.1994. Peer interactions in Polynesia: a view from the Marquesas. In Children’s Play in Diverse Cultures, eds. Roopnarine, J. L., Johnson, J. E. & Hooper, F. H.. Albany: State University of New York Press, 73–103.Google Scholar
Matevia, M. L., Patterson, F. & Hillix, W. A.2002. Pretend play in a signing gorilla, in Pretending and Imagination in Animals and Children, ed. Mitchell, R. W.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 285–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R.2007. Pretense in animals: the continuing relevance of children’s pretense, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 51–75.Google Scholar
Morris, D.1962. The Biology of Art. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A.2006. The interplay of play and narrative in children’s development: theoretical reflections and concrete examples, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 247–73.Google Scholar
Overton, W. F. & Jackson, J. P.1973. The representation of imagines objects in action sequences: a developmental study. Child Development, 44, 309–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peller, L. E.1954. Libidinal phases, ego development and play. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 9, 178–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Ruffman, T. & Leekam, S. R.1994. Theory of mind is contagious: you catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perone, A. & Göncü, A.2014. Life-span pretend play in two communities. Mind, Culture and Activity, 21(3), 200–20.
Piaget, J.1951. Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C.2007. Prehistory: The Making of the Human Mind. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S.1986. Ape Language: From Conditioned Response to Symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Slaughter, D. & Dombrowski, J.1989. Cultural continuities and discontinuities: impact on social and pretend play, in The Ecological Context of Children’s Play, eds. Bloch, M. N. & Pellegrini, A. D.. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 282–310.Google Scholar
Smilansky, S.1968. The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Preschool Children. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K.1988. Children’s play and its role in early development: a re-evaluation of the ‘play ethos’, in Psychological Bases for Early Education, ed. Pellegrini, A. D.. Chichester & New York: Wiley, 207–26.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K.2010. Children and Play. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Takhvar, M. & Smith, P. K.1990. A review and critique of Smilansky’s classification scheme and the ‘nested hierarchy’ of play categories. Journal of Research in Early Childhood, 4, 112–22.Google Scholar
Taylor, M.1999. Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Cartwright, B. S. & Carlson, S. M.1993. A developmental investigation of children’s imaginary companions. Developmental Psychology, 29, 276–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolley, J. D. & Wellman, H. H.1990. Young children’s understanding of realities, nonrealities, and appearances. Child Development, 64, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
References
Alexander, R. (1989) Evolution of the human psyche. In Mellars, P. & Stringer, C. (eds.) The Human Revolution: Behavioural and Biological Perspectives on the Origins of Modern Humans. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 455–513.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. (2007) Gods. In Whitehouse, H. & Laidlaw, J. (eds.) Religion, Anthropology and Cognitive Science. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press, 105–32.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. (2005) The role of play in the evolution of great apes and humans. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 13–24.Google Scholar
Bermúdez de Castro, J., Martinón-Torres, M., Prado, L., Gómez-Robles, A., Rosell, J., López-Polin, L., Arsuaga, J. & Carbonell, E. (2010) New immature hominin fossil from European Lower Pleistocene shows the earliest evidence of a modern human dental development pattern. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences of the USA107, 11739–44.Google ScholarPubMed
Bogin, B. (2003) The human pattern of growth and development in palaeontological perspective. In Thompson, J., Krovitz, G. & Nelson, A. (eds.) Patterns of Growth and Development in the Genus Homo. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 15–44.Google Scholar
Boyd, B. (2009) On the Origin of Stories: Evolution, Cognition and Fiction. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. (1985) Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002) Human creativity: its cognitive basis, its evolution, and its connections with childhood pretence. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science53, 225–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coqueugniot, H., Hublin, J.-J., Veillon, F., Houët, F. & Jacob, T. (2004) Early brain growth in Homo erectus and implications for cognitive ability. Nature431, 299–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, P., Zhang, S., Winkworth, A. & Bandler, R. (1996) Neural control of vocalisation: respiratory and emotional influences. Journal of Voice10, 23–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dean, C., Leakey, M., Reid, D., Schrenk, F., Schwartz, G., Stringer, C. & Walker, A. (2001) Growth processes in teeth distinguish modern humans from Homo erectus and earlier hominins. Nature414, 628–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSilva, J. & Lesnik, J. (2006) Chimpanzee neonatal brain size: implications for brain growth in Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution51, 207–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donald, M. (1991) Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (2001) A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. London: Norton.Google Scholar
Finke, R., Ward, T. & Smith, S. (1992) Creative Cognition. Boston: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fry, D. (2005) Rough and tumble social play in humans. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 54–85.Google Scholar
Gaskins, S. (1999) Children’s lives in a Mayan village: a case of culturally constructed roles and activities. In Gönkü, A. (ed.) Children’s Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 25–61.Google Scholar
Geary, D. & Bjorklund, D. (2000) Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development71, 57–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, J.-C. & Martin-Andrade, B. (2005) Fantasy play in apes. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 139–72.Google Scholar
Gosso, Y., Otta, E., Morais, M., Ribeiro, F. & Bussab, V. (2005) Play in hunter-gatherer society. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 213–53.Google Scholar
Graves, R., Lupo, A., McCarthy, R., Wescott, D. & Cunningham, D. (2010) Just how strapping was KNM-WT 15000?Journal of Human Evolution59, 542–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guatelli-Steinberg, D. (2009) Recent studies of dental development in Neanderthals: implications for Neanderthal life histories. Evolutionary Anthropology18, 9–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guthrie, S. (2007) Anthropology and anthropomorphism in religion. In Whitehouse, H. & Laidlaw, J. (eds.) Religion, Anthropology and Cognitive Science. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press, 37–62.Google Scholar
Haight, W. & Miller, P. (1993) Pretending at Home: Early Development in a Sociocultural Perspective. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., Jones, N. B., Alvarez, H. & Charnov, E. L. (1998) Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences95, 1336–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hochberg, Z. (2008) Juvenility in the context of life history theory. Archives of Disease in Childhood93, 534–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V. & Whiten, A. (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition8, 164–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jürgens, U. (1992) On the neurobiology of vocal communication. In Papousek, H., Jürgens, U. & Papousek, M. (eds.) Nonverbal Vocal Communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 31–42.Google Scholar
Leigh, S. (2001) Evolution of human growth. Evolutionary Anthropology10, 223–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leigh, S. (2006) Brain ontogeny and life history in Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution50, 104–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, K. (2005) Social play in the great apes. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 27–53.Google Scholar
Macchiarelli, R., Bondioli, L., Debénath, A., Mazurier, A., Tournepiche, J.-F., Birch, W. & Dean, C. (2006) How Neanderthal molar teeth grew. Nature444, 748–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martín-González, , Mateos, J., Goikoetxea, A., Leonard, I., , W. & Rodríguez, J. (2012) Differences between Neanderthal and modern human infant and child growth models. Journal of Human Evolution63, 140–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConachie, B. (2011) An evolutionary perspective on play, performance and ritual. TDR: The Drama Review55, 33–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. (2007) Pretense in animals: the continuing relevance of children’s pretense. In Gönkü, A. & Gaskins, S. (eds.) Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives. London: Psychology Press, 51–75.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2012) Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture?Journal of Comparative Psychology126, 170–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M. & Blank, C. (2011) Imitation in children: when who gets copied is more important that what gets copied. Developmental Psychology47, 1050–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connell, C. & DeSilva, J. (2013) Mojokerto revisited: evidence for an intermediate pattern of brain growth in Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution65, 156–61.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K. & Jones, N. B. (1999) Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution36, 461–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olejniczak, A., Smith, T., Feeney, R., Macchiarelli, R., Mazurier, A., Bondioli, L., Rosas, A., Fortea, J., de la Rasilla, M., Garcia-Tabernero, A., Radovčić, J., Skinner, M., Toussaint, M. & Hublin, J.-J. (2008) Dental tissue proportions and enamel thickness in Neanderthal and modern human molars. Journal of Human Evolution55, 12–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peccei, J. S. (2001) A critique of the grandmother hypotheses: old and new. American Journal of Human Biology13, 434–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (2005) Play in great apes and humans. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 3–12.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (2005) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Ruffman, T. & Leekam, S. (1994) Theory of mind is contagious: you catch it from your sibs. Child Development65, 1228–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ponce de León, M., Golovanova, L., Doronichev, V., Romanova, G., Akazawa, T., Kondo, O., Ishida, H. & Zollikofer, C. (2008) Neanderthal brain size at birth provides insights into the evolution of human life history. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA105, 13764–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rightmire, G. (2004) Brain size and encephalization in early to mid-Pleistocene Homo. American Journal of Physical Anthropology124, 109–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robson, S. & Wood, B. (2008) Hominin life history: reconstruction and evolution. Journal of Anatomy212, 394–425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartzmann, H. (1978). Transformations: The Anthropology of Children’s Play. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Slaughter, D. & Dombrowski, J. (1989). Cultural continuities and discontinuities: impact on social and pretend play. In Bloch, M. & Pellegrini, A. (eds.) The Ecological Context of Children’s Play. Nowood: Ablex, 282–310.Google Scholar
Smith, B. & Tompkins, R. (1995) Towards a life history of the hominidae. Annual Review of Anthropology24, 257–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. (1982) Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioural and Brain Sciences5, 139–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. (2005) Social and pretend play in children. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 173–209.Google Scholar
Smith, P. (2007) Evolutionary foundations and functions of play: an overview. In Gönkü, A. & Gaskins, S. (eds.) Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives. London: Psychology Press, 21–49.Google Scholar
Smith, P. (2010) Children and Play. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Smith, P. & Pellegrini, A. (2005) Play in great apes and humans: reflections on continuities and discontinuities. In Pellegrini, A. & Smith, P. (eds.) The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. London: Guilford Press, 285–98.Google Scholar
Smith, T., Tafforeau, P., Reid, D., Grün, R., Eggins, S., Boutakiout, M. & Hublin, J.-J. (2007) Earliest evidence of modern human life history in North African early Homo sapiens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA104, 6128–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, T., Tafforeau, P., Reid, D., Pouech, J., Lazzari, V., Zermeno, J., Guatelli-Stainberg, D., Olejniczak, A., Hoffman, A., Radovčić, J., Makaremi, M., Toussaint, M., Stringer, C. & Hublin, J.-J. (2010) Dental evidence for ontogenetic differences between modern humans and Neanderthals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA107, 20923–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, J. & Nelson, A. (2011) Middle childhood and modern human origins. Human Nature22, 249–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1999) The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vinicius, L. (2005) Human encephalization and developmental timing. Journal of Human Evolution49, 762–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, A. & Ruff, C. (1993) The reconstruction of the pelvis. In Walker, A. & Leakey, R. (eds.) The Nariokotome Homo erectus Skeleton. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2005) The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature437, 52–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zollikofer, C. & Ponce de León, M. (2009) The evolution of hominin ontogenies. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology21, 441–52.Google ScholarPubMed
References
Alland, A., Jr. 1983. Playing With Form. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Bartels, A. & Zeki, S., 2004. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage21(3), 1155–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F. & Leary, M. R., 1995. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin117(3), 497–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, J. & Forstmeier, W., 2007. Superstition and belief as inevitable by-products of an adaptive learning strategy. Human Nature18(1), 35–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bednarik, R., 2008. Cupules. Rock Art Research25, 61–100.Google Scholar
Bernieri, F. J. & Rosenthal, R., 1991. Interpersonal coordination: behaviour matching and interactional synchrony, in Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behaviour, eds. Feldman, R. S. & Rimé, B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 401–32.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. & Pellegrini, A. D., 2002. The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouzouggar, A., Barton, N., Vanhaeren, M., d’Errico, F., Collcutt, S., Higham, T. et al. 2007. 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origins of modern human behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences104(24), 9964–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S. & Dissanayake, E., 2009. The arts are more than aesthetics: neuroaesthetics as narrow aesthetics, in Neuroaesthetics, eds. Skov, M. & Vartanian, O.. Amityville, NY: Baywood, 43–57.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrill, R., 2010. The primacy of movement in art making. Teaching Artist Journal 8, 216–228.
Caporael, L. R., 1997. The evolution of truly social cognition: the core configuration model. Personality and Social Psychology Review1, 276–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, C. S., 1998. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology23, 779–818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. & Altemus, M., 1999. Integrative functions of lactational hormones in social behaviour and stress management, in The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation, eds. Carter, C., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 361–71.Google Scholar
Carter, C., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B., (eds.), 1999. The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Charmove, A. S. & Anderson, J. R., 1989. Examining environmental enrichment, in Housing, Care and Psychological Well Being of Captive and Laboratory Animals, ed. Segal, E. F.. Park Ridge, NJ: Noyes Publications, 183–202.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A., 1999. The chameleon effect: the perception-behaviour link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology76, 893–910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. & Dickinson, A., 2003. Can animals recall the past and plan for the future?Nature Reviews Neuroscience4, 685–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., 2000. Consider the source: the evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation, in Metarepresentations, ed. Sperber, D.. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 53–115.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., 1994. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Grosset Putnam.Google Scholar
d’Errico, F., Verhaeren, M., Barton, N., Bouzouggar, A., Mienis, H., Richter, D. et al. 2009. Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Paleolithic of North Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences106(38), 16051–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Dissanayake, E., 1979. An ethological view of ritual and art in human evolutionary history. Leonardo12(1), 27–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 1988. What Is Art For?Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 1999. Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother–infant interaction, in The Origins of Music, eds. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B. & Brown, S.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 389–410.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2000. Art and Intimacy: How the Arts Began. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2001. Becoming Homo aestheticus: sources of aesthetic imagination in mother–infant interactions. SubStance30(1,2), 85–103.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2006. Ritual and ritualization: musical means of conveying and shaping emotion in humans and other animals, in Music and Manipulation: On the Social Uses and Social Control of Music, eds. Brown, S. & Volgsten, U.. Oxford: Berghahn, 31–57.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2008. The arts after Darwin: does art have an origin and adaptive function?, in World Art Studies: Exploring Concepts and Approaches, eds. Zijlmans, K. & Damme, W. van. Amsterdam: Valiz, 241–63.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2016). Mark-making as a human behavior, in Darwin’s Bridge: Uniting the Humanities and Social Sciences, eds. Carroll, J., McAdams, D. P., & Wilson, E. O.. New York: Oxford University Press, 101–30.Google Scholar
Donald, M., 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Cognition and Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dugatkin, L. A., 1997. Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., Kaskatis, K., MacDonald, I. & Barra, V., 2012. Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evolutionary Psychology10(4): 688–702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I.1971. Love and Hate: The Natural History of Behaviour Patterns, translated by Geoffrey Strachan. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I.1989. Human Ethology, translated by Pauline Wiessner-Larsen and Anette Heunemann. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P., 1991. Some implications of cognitive appraisal theories of emotion, in International Review of Studies of Emotion, vol. 1, ed. Strongman, K.. New York: Wiley, 143–61.Google Scholar
Falk, D., 2004. Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese?Behavioural and Brain Sciences27(4), 491–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, D., 2009. Finding Our Tongues: Mothers, Infants & The Origin of Language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Feierman, J. R. (ed.), 2009. The Biology of Religious Behaviour: The Evolutionary Origins of Faith and Religion. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.Google Scholar
Fein, S., 1993. First Drawings: Genesis of Visual Thinking. Pleasant Hill, CA: Exelrod Press.Google Scholar
Flinn, M. V., Quinlan, R. J., Decker, S. A., Turner, M. T. & England, B. G., 1996. Male-female differences in effects of parental absence on glucocorticoid stress response. Human Nature7(2), 125–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flinn, M. V. & Ward, C. V., 2005. Evolution of the social child, in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development, eds. Ellis, B. & Bjorklund, D.. London: Guilford Press, 19–44.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., 2005. Folk knowledge and academic learning, in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development, eds. Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D.F.. New York: Guilford, 493–519.Google Scholar
Gianino, A. & Tronick, E. Z., 1988. The mutual regulation model: the infant’s self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive capacities, in Stress and Coping, eds. Field, T., McCabe, P. M. & Schneiderman, N.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 47–68.Google Scholar
Guss, D. M., 1998. To Weave and Sing: Art, Symbol, and Narrative in the South American Rain Forest. Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J. R., 1995. Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review102, 458–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazan, C. & Zeifman, D., 1999. Pair bonds as attachments: evaluating the evidence, in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, eds. Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R.. New York: Guilford, 336–54.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C. & Ehlert, U., 2003. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry54, 1389–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, G. A., et al. 2002. Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science295(5558), 1278–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R., 1975. Biological Bases of Human Social Relationships. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Hinde, R., 1982. Ethology: Its Nature and Relations With Other Sciences. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hofer, M. A., 1987. Early social relationships: a psychobiologist’s view. Child Development58, 633–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, E. A., Brewin, C. R. & Hennessy, R. G., 2004. Trauma films, information processing, and intrusive memory development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General133(1), 3–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huether, G., Doering, S., Rueger, U. & Ruether, E., 1996. Psychic stress and neuronal plasticity: an expanded model of the stress reaction processes as basis for the understanding of adaptive processes in the central nervous system. Zeitschrift für Psychosomatische Medizin und Psychoanalyse42, 107–27.Google Scholar
Huxley, J., 1914. The courtship habits of the great crested grebe (Podiceps cristatus) together with a discussion of the evolution of courtship in birds. Journal of the Linnean Society of London: Zoology53, 253–92.Google Scholar
Huxley, J., 1966. Introduction, in A Discussion on Ritualisation of Behaviour in Animals and Man, organized by Sir Julian Huxley. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. Biological Sciences 772, 249–71.
Kaptchuk, T. J., Kerr, C. E. & Zanger, A., 2009. Placebo controls, exorcisms, and the devil. Lancet374 (9697), 1234–5. (10 Oct.).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J. & May, J., 2005. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: the elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review112(2), 446–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. & Edwards, K., 1993. Beyond simple pessimism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology64, 740–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, B. J., 2004. The Dynamic Dance: Nonvocal Communication in African Great Apes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Koelsch, S., Offermanns, K. & Franzke, P., 2010. Music in the treatment of affective disorders: an exploratory investigation of a new method for music-therapeutic research. Music Perception27(4), 307–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leakey, R., 1994. The Origin of Humankind. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M., 1987. Pretense and representation: origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review94, 412–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lillard, A. S., 1993. Pretend play skills and the child’s theory of mind. Child Development64(2), 348–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopreato, J., 1984. Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution. Boston, MA: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. Z., 1982. The Foundations of Ethology: The Principal Ideas and Discoveries in Animal Behaviour, translated by K. Z. Lorenz and R. W. Kickert. (Original German publication 1981). New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B., 1922. Argonauts of the Western Pacific. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B., 1927. The life of culture, in Culture: The Diffusion Controversy, eds. Smith, G. E., Malinowski, B., Spinden, H. J. & Goldenweiser, A.New York: Norton, 26–46.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B., 1948. Magic, Science, and Religion. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. Original publication 1925.Google Scholar
Mead, M.1976. Growing Up in New Guinea. New York: Morrow. Original publication 1930.Google Scholar
Meyer-Holzapfel, M., 1956. Das Spiel bei Säugetieren. Handbuch der Zoologie8, 1–26.Google Scholar
Miller, W. B. & Rodgers, J. L., 2001. The Ontogeny of Human Bonding Systems: Evolutionary Origins, Neural Bases, and Psychological Manifestations. Boston, MA and Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S., 2005. The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Morris, D., 1957. ‘Typical intensity’ and its relation to the problem of ritualisation, Behaviour11, 1–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. E. & Panksepp, J., 1998. Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews22, 437–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakley, K. P., 1971. Fossil shell observed by Acheulian Man. Antiquity47, 59–60.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., 1998. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundation of Animal and Human Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Biven, L., 2012. The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Nelson, E. & Bekkedal, M., 1999. Brain systems for the mediation of social separation-distress and social-reward: evolutionary antecedents and neuropeptide intermediaries, in The Neurobiology of Affiliation, eds. Carter, C. S., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 222–41.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. & Bjorklund, D. F., 2004. The ontogeny and phylogeny of children’s object and fantasy play. Human Nature15(1), 23–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips-Silver, J., Aktipis, C. A. & Bryant, G. A., 2010. The ecology of entrainment: foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Perception28(1), 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips-Silver, J. & Keller, P. E.. 2012. Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience6(26), 1–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raby, C. R., Alexis, D. M., Dickinson, A. & Clayton, N. S., 2007. Planning for the future by Western scrub-jays. Nature445, 919–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radcliffe-Brown, A. R., 1952. Religion and society, in Structure and Function in Primitive Society. Glencoe, IL: Free Press, 153–77. Original publication in Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (1945) 75(1–2), 33–43.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A., 1999. Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, K. R., 1992. The evolution of modern human childbirth. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology35, 89–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M., 1992. Neuroendocrinology of the stress response, in Behavioural Endocrinology, eds. Becker, J. R., Breedlove, S. M. & Crews, D.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 287–324.Google Scholar
Schechner, R., 2002. Performance Studies: An Introduction. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Schiefenhövel, W., 2009. Explaining the inexplicable: traditional and syncretistic religiosity in Melanesia, in The Biological Evolution of Religious Mind and Behaviour, eds. Voland, E. & Schiefenhövel, W.. Berlin: Springer, 143–64.Google Scholar
Shaver, P. R., Hazan, C. & Bradshaw, D., 1988. Love as attachment: the integration of three behavioral systems, in The Anatomy of Love, eds. Sternberg, R. J. & Barnes, M.. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, W., 1977. The Behavior of Communicating: An Evolutionary Approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D.1977. The First Relationship. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M.C., 1997. Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs123, 133–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Tambiah, S. J., 1979. A performative approach to ritual. Proceedings of the British Academy, London LXV, 113–69. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, S., 1992. The Tending Instinct: How Nurturing Is Essential to Who We Are and How We Live. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., 2007. Social support, in Foundations of Health Psychology, eds. H. S. Friedman & R. C. Silver. New York: Oxford University Press, 145–71.
Taylor, S. E., Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I., Lehman, B. J., Hilmeet, C. J. & Lieberman, M. D., 2008. Neural bases of moderation of cortisol stress responses by psychosocial resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology95(1), 197–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tinbergen, N., 1952. ‘Derived’ activities: their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology17, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N., 1959. Comparative studies of the behaviour of gulls (Laridae): a progress report. Behaviour15, 1–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonkinson, R., 1978. The Mardudjara Aborigines: Living the Dream in Australia’s Desert. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L., 2001. Does beauty build adapted minds? Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction, and the arts. SubStance30(1,2), 6–27.Google Scholar
Turner, V., 1969. The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Uvnäs-Moberg, K., 1999. Physiological and endocrine effects of social contact, in The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation, eds. Carter, C., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 245–61.Google Scholar
Van Gennep, A., 1960. The Rites of Passage. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Original work published in 1908.Google Scholar
Vanhaeren, M., d'Errico, F., Stringer, C., James, S. L., Todd, J. A., & H. K. Mienis. 2006. Middle paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 312 (5781), 1785–8.
Watanabe, J. M. & Smuts, B. B., 1999. Explaining religion without explaining it away: trust, truth, and the evolution of cooperation in Roy A. Rappaport’s ‘The obvious aspects of ritual’. American Anthropologist101, 98–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whybrow, P., 1984. Contributions from neuroendocrinology, in Approaches to Emotion, eds. Scherer, K. & Ekman, P.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 59–72.Google Scholar
Yellen, J. E., Brooks, A. S., Cornelissen, E., Mehlman, M. J. & Stewart, K., 1995. A Middle Stone Age worked bone industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. Science268 (28 April), 553–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed