Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-05T17:34:02.887Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Parenting, Challenges, Brain Development, and Attachment Strategies

from Part I - Foundations of Parenting

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 2022

Amanda Sheffield Morris
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University
Julia Mendez Smith
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Greensboro
Get access

Summary

Human infants are born needing their caregivers’ support to accomplish challenges related to both security and exploration. Accordingly, the quality of care infants receive influences their ability to appraise the degree of threat inherent to any challenge, signal needs for assistance, and regulate their responses. In this manner, parenting affects whether young children manage challenges with behavior or physiological responses. The extent to which stress physiology is repeatedly invoked in response to challenges, alongside variation in neural growth accompanying children’s exploratory behavior, in turn affects neurodevelopment and ultimately functioning with age. We discuss the processes through which this occurs, the potential impact on attachment schemas, and implications for intervention programs designed at improving parenting and well-being.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahnert, L., Gunnar, M. R., Lamb, M. E., & Barthel, M. (2004). Transition to child care: Associations with infant–mother attachment, infant negative emotion, and cortisol elevations. Child Development, 75, 639650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709716. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Atkinson, L., Gonzalez, A., Kashy, D. A. et al. (2013). Maternal sensitivity and infant and mother adrenocortical function across challenges. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 29432951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.08.001Google Scholar
Aviezer, O., Sagi-Schwartz, A., & Koren-Karie, N. (2003). Ecological constraints on the formation of infant–mother attachment relations: When maternal sensitivity becomes ineffective. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 285299.Google Scholar
Aviezer, O., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Sagi, A., & Schuengel, C. (1994). “Children of the dream” revisited: 70 years of collective early child care in Israeli kibbutzim. Psycholical Bulletin, 116, 99116. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.99Google Scholar
Bagot, R. C., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological basis of gene x environment interactions. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 752771. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.06.001Google Scholar
Baker, L., Sonnenschein, S., & Gilat, M. (1996). Mothers’ sensitivity to the competencies of their preschoolers on a concept-learning task. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 405424. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2006(96)90014-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Mesman, J., Alink, L. R., & Juffer, F. (2008a). Effects of an attachment-based intervention on daily cortisol moderated by dopamine receptor D4: A randomized control trial on 1- to 3-year-olds screened for externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 805820. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579408000382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pijlman, F. T., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008b). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 44, 293300.Google Scholar
Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2006). Gene‐environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 406409.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (1997) Early day care and infant-mother attachment security. Bennett J, topic ed. In R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin and R. Peters (Eds.), Encyclopedia on early childhood development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development and Strategic Knowledge Cluster on Early Child Development, pp. 1–6. https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.567.1008&rep=rep1&type=pdfGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Garduque, L., & Hrncir, E. (1984). Assessing performance, competence, and executive capacity in infant play: Relations to home environment and security of attachment. Developmental Psychology, 20, 406417. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.3.406Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647670. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131166Google Scholar
Benoit, D. (2004). Infant-parent attachment: Definition, types, antecedents, measurement and outcome. Paediatrics & Child Health, 9, 541545.Google Scholar
Bernier, A., Dégeilh, F., Leblanc, É., Daneault, V., Bailey, H. N., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2019). Mother–infant interaction and child brain morphology: A multidimensional approach to maternal sensitivity. Infancy, 24, 120138. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12270Google Scholar
Bernier, A., & Meins, E. (2008). A threshold approach to understanding the origins of attachment disorganization. Devopmental Psychology, 44, 969982. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.969CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biro, S., Alink, L. R. A., Huffmeijer, R., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., & Ijzendoorn, M. H. V. (2015). Attachment and maternal sensitivity are related to infants’ monitoring of animated social interactions. Brain and Behavior, 5, 113. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.410Google Scholar
Blair, C., Granger, D., Willoughby, M., & Kivlighan, K. (2006). Maternal sensitivity is related to hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis stress reactivity and regulation in response to emotion challenge in 6-month-old infants. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 263267. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.031Google Scholar
Bosmans, G., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Vervliet, B., Verhees, M., & van Ijzendoom, I. M. H. (2020). A learning theory of attachment: Unraveling the black box of attachment development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 113, 287298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.014Google Scholar
Bosmans, G., Young, J. F., & Hankin, B. L. (2018). NR3C1 methylation as a moderator of the effects of maternal support and stress on insecure attachment development. Developmental Psychology, 54, 2938.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 39, 350373.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss (Vol. 1). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss (Vol. 2 Separation: Anxiety & Anger). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 525.Google Scholar
Carr, Sam, and Landau, Sean. 2012. Consciously Identified Attachment Hierarchies: Cognitive Accessibility of Attachment Figure Names as a Function of Threat Primes in a Lexical Decision Task. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 1725. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00916.x.Google Scholar
Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. J. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of attachment: Theory and research. Child Development, 65, 971981. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131298Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Ashman, S. B., Hessl, D. et al. (2001). Autonomic and brain electrical activity in securely- and insecurely-attached infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior & Development, 24, 135149. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00075-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, G., Klinger, L. G., Panagiotides, H., Spieker, S., & Frey, K. (1992). Infants of mothers with depressive symptoms: Electroencephalographic and behavioral findings related to attachment status. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 6780. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The Adaptive Calibration Model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 15621592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007Google Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lewis, E., Laurenceau, J. P., & Levine, S. (2008). Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Developmental Psychopathology, 20, 845859. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579408000400Google Scholar
Dunn, E. C., Busso, D. S., Raffeld, M. R. et al. (2016). Does developmental timing of exposure to child maltreatment predict memory performance in adulthood? Results from a large, population-based sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 51, 181191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.10.014Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Developmental Psychopathology, 23, 728. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579410000611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., Nelson, C. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2017). The effects of psychosocial deprivation on attachment: Lessons from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Psychodynamic Psychiatry, 45, 441450. https://doi.org/10.1521/pdps.2017.45.4.441Google Scholar
Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., & Haltigan, J. D. (2013). The legacy of early experiences in development: Formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology, 49, 109126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a002785210.1037/a0027852.supp (Supplemental)Google Scholar
Frodi, A., Bridges, L., & Grolnick, W. (1985). Correlates of mastery-related behavior: A short-term longitudinal study of infants in their second year. Child Development, 56, 12911298.Google Scholar
Garrett-Peters, P. T., & Fox, N. A. (2007). Cross-cultural differences in children’s emotional reactions to a disappointing situation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 161169.Google Scholar
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J. et al. (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala–prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 1563815643. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1307893110Google Scholar
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L. et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 81748179. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0402680101Google Scholar
Granqvist, P., Hesse, E., Fransson, M., Main, M., Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (2016). Prior participation in the strange situation and overstress jointly facilitate disorganized behaviours: Implications for theory, research and practice. Attachment & Human Development, 18, 235249. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1151061Google Scholar
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. M. P., IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Roisman, G. I. (2017). Attachment in the early life course: Meta-analytic evidence for its role in socioemotional development. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 7076. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12213Google Scholar
Haley, D. W., & Stansbury, K. (2003). Infant stress and parent responsiveness: Regulation of physiology and behavior during still‐face and reunion. Child Development, 74, 15341546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hane, A., & Fox, N. (2016). Studying the biology of human attachment. In Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 223241). The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Deeper into attachment theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 6879.Google Scholar
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2000). Disorganized infant, child, and adult attachment: Collapse in behavioral and attentional strategies. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48, 10971127. https://doi.org/10.1177/00030651000480041101Google Scholar
Humphreys, K. L., McGoron, L., Sheridan, M. A. et al. (2015). High-quality foster care mitigates callous-unemotional traits following early deprivation in boys: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 977983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.09.010Google Scholar
Jennings, K. D., Harmon, R. J., Morgan, G. A., Gaiter, J. L., & Yarrow, L. J. (1979). Exploratory play as an index of mastery motivation: Relationships to persistence, cognitive functioning, and environmental measures. Developmental Psychology, 15, 386394. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.15.4.386Google Scholar
Jones‐Mason, K., Allen, I. E., Bush, N., Hamilton, S., (2006) Epigenetic marks as the link between environment and development: Examination of the associations between attachment, socioeconomic status, and methylation of the SLC6A4 gene. Brain and Behavior, 6, e00480. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.480Google Scholar
Kermoian, R., & Leiderman, P. H. (1986). Infant attachment to mother and child caretaker in an East African community. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 455469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kok, R., Thijssen, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. et al. (2015). Normal variation in early parental sensitivity predicts child structural brain development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 824831. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.07.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kondo-Ikemura, K., Behrens, K. Y., Umemura, T., & Nakano, S. (2018). Japanese mothers’ prebirth Adult Attachment Interview predicts their infants’ response to the Strange Situation Procedure: The strange situation in Japan revisited three decades later. Developmental Psychololgy, 54, 20072015. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000577Google Scholar
Kovan, N. M., Chung, A. L., & Sroufe, L. A. (2009). The intergenerational continuity of observed early parenting: A prospective, longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45, 12051213. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016542Google Scholar
Kungl, M. T., Leyh, R., & Spangler, G. (2016). Attachment representations and brain asymmetry during the processing of autobiographical emotional memories in late adolescence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 644. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00644Google Scholar
Lee, A., Poh, J. S., Wen, D. J. et al. (2019). Maternal Care in infancy and the course of limbic development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 40, 100714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100714Google Scholar
Leyh, R., Heinisch, C., Kungl, M. T., & Spangler, G. (2016). Attachment representation moderates the influence of emotional context on information processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Belden, A., Harms, M. P., Tillman, R., & Barch, D. M. (2016). Preschool is a sensitive period for the influence of maternal support on the trajectory of hippocampal development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 113, 57425747. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601443113Google Scholar
Luijk, M. P., Roisman, G. I., Haltigan, J. D. et al. (2011). Dopaminergic, serotonergic, and oxytonergic candidate genes associated with infant attachment security and disorganization? In search of main and interaction effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 12951307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02440.xGoogle Scholar
Lupien, S. J., Ouellet-Morin, I., Hupbach, A. et al. (2006). Beyond the stress concept: Allostatic load – a developmental biological and cognitive perspective. In Developmental psychopathology: Developmental neuroscience, Vol. 2, 2nd ed. (pp. 578628). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Pechtel, P., Yoon, S. A., Anderson, C. M., & Teicher, M. H. (2016). Disorganized attachment in infancy predicts greater amygdala volume in adulthood. Behavioural Brain Research, 308, 8393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.03.050Google Scholar
Madigan, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Moran, G., Pederson, D. R., & Benoit, D. (2006). Unresolved states of mind, anomalous parental behavior, and disorganized attachment: A review and meta-analysis of a transmission gap. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 89111. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730600774458Google Scholar
Main, M. (1981). Avoidance in the service of attachment. In Immelman K, B. G.., Petrinovitch, L., & Main, M. (Ed.), Behavioral development (pp. 651693). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Main, M. (2000). The organized categories of infant, child, and adult attachment: Flexible vs inflexible attention under attachment-related stress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48, 10551096. https://doi.org/10.1177/00030651000480041801Google Scholar
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism?. In Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (Vol. xix 507, pp. 161182). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In (pp. 121160). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Matte-Gagne, C., Bernier, A., Sirois, M. S., Lalonde, G., & Hertz, S. (2018). Attachment security and developmental patterns of growth in executive functioning during early elementary school. Child Development, 89, e167e182. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12807Google Scholar
McEwen, B. S., Nasca, C., & Gray, J. D. (2016). Stress effects on neuronal structure: Hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41, 323. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.171Google Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). Attachment security as a mechanism linking foster care placement to improved mental health outcomes in previously institutionalized children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53, 4655. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02437.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merz, E. C., Landry, S. H., Zucker, T. A. et al. (2016). Parenting predictors of delay inhibition in socioeconomically disadvantaged preschoolers. Infant and Child Development, 25, 371390. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1946Google Scholar
Miller, D. J., Duka, T., Stimpson, C. D. et al. (2012). Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 1648016485. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117943109Google Scholar
Moss, E., & St-Laurent, D. (2001). Attachment at school age and academic performance. Developmental Psychology, 37, 863874. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moutsiana, C., Fearon, P., Murray, L. et al. (2014). Making an effort to feel positive: Insecure attachment in infancy predicts the neural underpinnings of emotion regulation in adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 9991008. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12198Google Scholar
Perry, R. E., Finegood, E. D., Braren, S. H. et al. (2018). Developing a neurobehavioral animal model of poverty: Drawing cross-species connections between environments of scarcity-adversity, parenting quality, and infant outcome. Development and Psychopathology, 31, 399418. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941800007XGoogle Scholar
Raby, K. L., Roisman, G. I., Fraley, R. C., & Simpson, J. A. (2015). The enduring predictive significance of early maternal sensitivity: Social and academic competence through age 32 years. Child Development, 86, 695708. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12325Google Scholar
Rao, H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M. et al. (2010). Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: Evidence from brain morphology in humans. Neuroimage, 49, 11441150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rifkin-Graboi, A. (2008). Attachment status and salivary cortisol in a normal day and during simulated interpersonal stress in young men. Stress, 11, 210224. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890701706670Google Scholar
Rifkin-Graboi, A., Borelli, J., & Bosquest, M. (2009). Neurobiology of Stress in Infancy. In Zeanah, C. (Ed.), Handbook of Infant Mental Health (pp. 5979). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rifkin-Graboi, A., Kong, L., Sim, L. W. et al. (2015). Maternal sensitivity, infant limbic structure volume and functional connectivity: A preliminary study. Translational Psychiatry, 5, e668. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.133Google Scholar
Rifkin-Graboi, A., Quan, J., Richmond, J. et al. (2018). Greater caregiving risk, better infant memory performance? Hippocampus, 28, 497511. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22949Google Scholar
Rifkin-Graboi, A., Qiang, Qiu et al. (2019). Attachment Relationships Meet Working Models: The Meat of the Models. Paper presented at the International Attachment Conference, Vancouver, Canada.Google Scholar
Rifkin-Graboi, A., Tan, H. M., Shaun, G. K. Y. et al. (2019). An initial investigation of neonatal neuroanatomy, caregiving, and levels of disorganized behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116, 1678716792. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1900362116Google Scholar
Roisman, G. I., Booth-Laforce, C., Belsky, J., Burt, K. B., & Groh, A. M. (2013). Molecular-genetic correlates of infant attachment: A cautionary tale. Attachment and Human Development, 15, 384406. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013.768790Google Scholar
Roisman, G. I., Haltigan, J. D., Haydon, K. C., & Booth‐LaForce, C. (2014). The Adult Attachment Interview: Psychometrics, stability and change from infancy, and developmental origins: VI Earned‐security in retrospect: Depressive symptoms, family stress, and maternal and paternal sensitivity from early childhood to mid‐adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 85107. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12115Google Scholar
Rothbaum, F., Kakinuma, M., Nagaoka, R., & Azuma, H. (2007). Attachment and amae: Parent–child closeness in the United States and Japan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 465486.Google Scholar
Sagi, A., Koren-Karie, N., Gini, M., Ziv, Y., & Joels, T. (2002). Shedding further light on the effects of various types and quality of early child care on infant–mother attachment relationship: The Haifa Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 73, 11661186. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00465Google Scholar
Schieche, M., & Spangler, G. (2005). Individual differences in biobehavioral organization during problem‐solving in toddlers: The influence of maternal behavior, infant–mother attachment, and behavioral inhibition on the attachment‐exploration balance. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 293306.Google Scholar
Schnall, S., Harber, K. D., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Social support and the perception of geographical slant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 12461255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.011Google Scholar
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., McCullough, A. M., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2017). The effects of acute stress on episodic memory: A meta-analysis and integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 636675. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000100Google Scholar
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 651668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038Google Scholar
Silvers, J. A., Lumian, D. S., Gabard-Durnam, L. et al. (2016). Previous institutionalization is followed by broader amygdala-hippocampal-pfc network connectivity during aversive learning in human development. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 64206430. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0038-16.2016Google Scholar
Simpson, J., & Kelly, J. P. (2011). The impact of environmental enrichment in laboratory rats – behavioural and neurochemical aspects. Behavioural Brain Research, 222, 246264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.002Google Scholar
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The development of attachment in separated and divorced families: Effects of overnight visitation, parent and couple variables. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 233. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616739900134011Google Scholar
Soltis, J. (2004) The signal functions of early infant crying. Behavioural Brain Science 27, 443490.Google Scholar
Sroufe, A., & McIntosh, J. (2011). Divorce and attachment relationships: The longitudinal journey. Family Court Review, 49, 464473.Google Scholar
Steele, R. D., Waters, T. E. A., Bost, K. K. et al. (2014). Caregiving antecedents of secure base script knowledge: A comparative analysis of young adult attachment representations. Developmental Psychology, 50, 25262538. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037992%2010.1037/a0037992.supp (Supplemental)Google Scholar
Stockhorst, U., & Antov, M. I. (2016). Modulation of fear extinction by stress, stress hormones and estradiol: A review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 359.Google Scholar
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34, 26552666. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.103Google Scholar
Sullivan, R. M., & Perry, R. E. (2015). Mechanisms and functional implications of social buffering in infants: Lessons from animal models. Social Neuroscience, 10, 500511. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1087425Google Scholar
Suomi, S. J. (2016). Attachment in Rhesus Monkeys. In Shaver, J. C. P. R. (Ed.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd Edition ed., pp. 133154). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, A. R., Raby, K. L., Goldstein, A., Zajac, L., & Dozier, M. (2019). Effects of an attachment-based intervention in infancy on children’s autonomic regulation during middle childhood. Biological Psychology, 143, 2231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.01.006Google Scholar
Thijssen, S., Muetzel, R. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. et al. (2017). Insensitive parenting may accelerate the development of the amygdala–medial prefrontal cortex circuit. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 505518. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000141Google Scholar
Tsotsi, S., Broekman, B. F. P., Sim, L. W. et al. (2019). Maternal anxiety, parenting stress, and preschoolers’ behavior problems: The role of child self-regulation. Journal of Developmental Behavioural Pediatrics, 40, 696705. https://doi.org/10.1097/dbp.0000000000000737Google Scholar
Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 397409. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2647Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Developmental Psychopatholy, 11, 225249.Google Scholar
Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S. et al. (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 337366. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul000003810.1037/bul0000038.supp (Supplemental)Google Scholar
Wang, Q., Zhang, H., Wee, C. Y. et al. (2019). Maternal sensitivity predicts anterior hippocampal functional networks in early childhood. Brain Structure and Function, 224, 18851895. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01882-0Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “the game.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 18, 323339.Google Scholar
Weinfield, N. S., Ogawa, J. R., & Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Early attachment as a pathway to adolescent peer competence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 7, 241265. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327795jra0703_1Google Scholar
Williford, A., Carter, L., & Pianta, R. (2016). Attachment and school readiness. J. Cassidy, & P. Shaver, Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications, 11, 966982.Google Scholar
Zeijlmans van Emmichoven, I. A.,van, I. M. H., de Ruiter, C., & Brosschot, J. F. (2003). Selective processing of threatening information: Effects of attachment representation and anxiety disorder on attention and memory. Developmental Psychopathology, 15, 219237. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579403000129Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×